Skip to main content

Serial Context Effects in Survey Interviews

  • Chapter

Abstract

In survey methods research, the context effects of preceding questions on responses to survey items have been the subject of many interesting studies (see, for instance, Abelson, 1984; Bishop, Oldendick, & Tuchfarber, 1985; Bradburn, 1983; Carpenter & Blackwood, 1979; McClendon & O’Brien, 1988; McFarland, 1981; Perreault, 1975; Schuman, Presser, & Ludwig, 1981; Schuman & Presser, 1981; Sigelman, 1981; T. W. Smith, 1981c; and the chapters in this volume). In particular, effects of question order have been investigated. In most of these studies, the effects of only one or two preceding questions were considered, or the focus was on part-whole combinations of questions. Schwarz and his associates also studied the effects on responses when different ranges of response categories are offered (e.g., Schwarz & Hippler, 1987).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Notes

  1. Therefore, we prefer to use the term “skewness effect” instead of “frequency effect.”

    Google Scholar 

  2. Only the size of the projection screen in these experiments might serve as a maximum.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Low and high probabilities were determined from mean ratings in the preliminary study.

    Google Scholar 

  4. In a control condition with equally spaced stimulus values and a full range, one half of the 40 respondents were presented the “left” order of focal stimuli, the other half the “right” order. There were no significant differences in mean ratings for the focal stimuli between respondents with one order of presentation and respondents with the other order (none of the F values came even close to significance). Therefore, we may conclude that differences in ratings due to range-frequency effects in this study are not confounded with order effects within the set of focal stimuli.

    Google Scholar 

  5. The number of refusals was 49; the remaining 63 did not cooperate because of other reasons (mostly “not at home”).

    Google Scholar 

  6. In this analysis, the ratings on the 9-point scale of conditions (LS9 and RS9) are linearly transformed into a scale from 1 to 5 to make these ratings comparable with those in LS5 and RS5.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Comparison of LS9 with LR (or of RS9 with RR) is less obvious because of the main effect of Number of Categories described above.

    Google Scholar 

  8. We expected range-skewness effects to be stronger in our face-to-face mode (with visual aid) than in the telephone mode. The major reason for this expectation was “the ability to see an entire set of questions in mail and other self-administered surveys … and the lack of this ability in telephone surveys” (Tarnai & Dillman, chap. 9, this volume). However, we found no significant mode effects.

    Google Scholar 

  9. By “frequentistic” stimuli, we mean events that occur in a repetitive fashion and thereby permit specification of likeliness in terms of relative frequency of occurrence. By nonfrequentistic stimuli, we mean events that, while uncertain, are non-repetitive and unique (Howell & Burnett, 1978).

    Google Scholar 

  10. As part of a follow-up experiment, we interviewed 40 respondents (out of the same pool of volunteers and with the same demographic characteristics as those in the present study). We asked them to judge the dissimilarity between focal and contextual Stimuli. More specifically, we asked them to pick 2 out of 10 focal stimuli that they believed to be the least similar to a specific contextual stimulus (7 of the 10 focal stimuli were the ones common to all conditions in the present study, the 3 other ones were relevant to the follow-up study). Respondents had to perform this task six times: They compared the 10 focal stimuli with 6 contextual stimuli (randomly chosen out of the contextual sets: there were 4 different samples of 6 stimuli, 2 drawn from the “left” contextual sets and 2 from the “right” sets). It appeared that the “opinion” focal stimuli are chosen most often as the most different from the contextual stimuli. These 3 focal stimuli (numbers 3, 5, and 6) were chosen significantly more often than chance (the z scores corrected for continuity are 2.67, 3.96, and 6.38, respectively, all p <.01).

    Google Scholar 

  11. We had our focal stimuli preceded by series of 30 or 32 contextual stimuli (depending on experimental conditions), but psychophysical experiments show that range-frequency effects also occur in shorter series.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1992 Springer-Verlag New York, Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Daamen, D.D.L., de Bie, S.E. (1992). Serial Context Effects in Survey Interviews. In: Schwarz, N., Sudman, S. (eds) Context Effects in Social and Psychological Research. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-2848-6_8

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-2848-6_8

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4612-7695-1

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4612-2848-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics