The Septal Complex as Seen Through the Context of Fear

  • Peter D. Sparks
  • Joseph E. LeDoux

Abstract

Over the past 15 years research concerning the neural basis of emotion has flourished. Progress has been especially swift in studies of the neural systems underlying fear (see Davis 1992; Kapp et al. 1992; Davis 1994; LeDoux 1996). Although there is still much to be known, the fear system stands as one of the most well-defined functional neural systems in the brain. Across studies using a variety of species, training conditions, and labs, one structure—the amygdala—has been consistently shown to play a critical role in the mediation of fear. To be specific, the amygdala has been shown to be critical for learning to fear previously neutral stimuli, and activating defensive behaviors in response to learned, as well as innate, threats (for review see LeDoux 1996). Nevertheless, other neural structures, such as the medial prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and septum, contribute to, or modulate, the processing of fear by the amygdala and the resulting expression of defensive behaviors as well (e.g., Blanchard and Fial 1968; Seiden et al. 1991; Kim and Fanselow 1992; Phillips and LeDoux 1992; Morgan and LeDoux 1993; Sparks and LeDoux 1995; Maren and Fanselow 1996; Frankland et al. 1998). This chapter examines the contribution of the septum to fear and the expression of defensive behaviors.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Ackil, J.E., Mellgren, R.L., Halgren, C, and Frommer, G.P. 1969. Effects of CS pre-exposures on avoidance learning in rats with hippocampal lesions. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 69:739–747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Adams, D.B. 1979. Brain mechanisms for offense, defence, and submission. Behav. Brain Sci. 2:201–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Albert, D.J., and Chew, G.L. 1980. The septal forebrain and the inhibitory modulation of attack and defense in the rat: a review. Behav. Neural Biol. 30:357–388.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Albert, D.J., and Richmond, S.E. 1975. Septal hyperactivity: a comparison of lesions within and adjacent to the septum. Physiol. Behav. 15:339–347.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Albert, DJ., and Walsh, M.L. 1982. The inhibitory modulation of agonistic behavior in the rat brain: a review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 6:125–143.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Amaral, D.G. 1987. Memory: anatomical organization of candidate brain regions. In Handbook of Physiology., Sect. 1: Neurophysiol., Vol 5: Higher Functions of the Brain., ed. F.Plum, pp. 211–294. Maryland:American Physiological SocieGoogle Scholar
  7. Amsel, A. 1993. Hippocampal function in the rat: cognitive mapping or vicarious trial and error? Hippocampus. 3:251–256.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Beatty, W.W., Beatty, B.A., O’Brian, D.H., Gregoire, K.C., and Dahl, B.L. 1973. Factors underlying deficient passive avoidance behavior by rats with septal lesions. J Comp. Physiol. Psych. 85:502–514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bengelloun, W.A., Burright, R.G., and Donovick, P. J. 1977. Septal lesions, cue availability, and passive avoidance acquisition by hooded male rats of two ages. Physiol Behav. 18:1033–1037.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Birke, L.I.A., and Archer, J. 1983. Some issues and problems in the study of animal exploration. In Exploration in Animals and Humans., eds. J. Archer, and L.I.A. Birke. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold Co.Google Scholar
  11. Blanchard, D.C., and Blanchard, R. J. 1972. Innate and conditioned reactions to threat in rats with amygdaloid lesions. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 81:281–290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Blanchard D.C., Blanchard, R. J., Takahashi, L.K., and Takahashi, T. 1977. Septal lesions and aggressive behavior. Behav. Biol. 21:157–161.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Blanchard, R. J., and Fial, R.A. 1968. Effects of limbic lesions on passive avoidance and reactivity to shock. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 66:606–612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Bliss, T.V.P., and Collingridge, G.L. 1993. A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature. 361:31–39.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Bolles, R.C., and Fanselow, M.S. 1980. A perceptual-defensive-recuperative model of fear and pain. Behav. Brain Sci. 3:291–323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Bouton, M.E., and Bolles, R.C. 1979. Contextual control of the extinction of conditioned fear. Learn. Motivation. 10:445–466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Brady, J.V., and Nauta, W. J.H. 1953. Subcortical mechanisms in emotional behavior: affective changes following septal forebrain lesions in the albino rat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 46:339–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Brady, J.V., and Nauta, W. J.H. 1955. Subcortical mechanisms in emotional behavior: the duration of affective changes following septal and habenular lesions in the albino rat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 48:412–420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Brown, T.H., Chapman, P.F., Kairiss, E.W., and Keenan, C.L. 1988. Long-term synaptic potentiation. Science. 242:724–728.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Caplan, M. 1973. An analysis of the effects of septal lesions on negatively reinforced behavior. Behav. Biol. 9:129–167.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Cole, J.C., and Rodgers, R. J. 1994. Ethological evaluation of the effects of acute and chronic buspirone treatment in the murine elevated plus-maze test: comparisons with haloperidol. Psychopharmacology. 114:288–296.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Davis, M. 1992. The role of the amygdala in conditioned fear. In The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction., ed. J.R Aggleton, pp. 255–306. New York: Wiley-Liss, Inc.Google Scholar
  23. Davis, M. 1994. The role of the amygdala in emotional learning. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 36:225–266.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Decker, M.W., Curzon, P., and Brioni, J.D. 1995. Influence of separate and combined septal and amygdala lesions on memory, acoustic startle, anxiety, and locomotor activity in rats. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 64:156–168.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Donovick, P.J., and Wakeman, K.A. 1969. Open-field luminance and septal hyper-emotionality. Animal Behav. 17:186–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Douglas, R.J. 1967. The hippocampus and behavior. Psychol. Bull. 67:416–442.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Douglas, R.J. 1972. Pavlovian conditioning and the brain. In Inhibition and Learning., eds. R.A. Boakes, and M.S. Halliday, pp. 529–553. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  28. Duncan, P.M. 1971. Effect of temporary septal dysfunction on conditioning and performance of fear responses in rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 74:340–348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Eichenbaum, H. 1997. Declarative memory: insights from cognitive neurobiology. Ann. Rev. Psych. 48:547–572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Eichenbaum, H., Cohen, N. J., Otto, T., and Wible, C. 1991. Memory representation in the hippocampus: functional domain and functional organization. In Memory: Organization and Locus of Change., eds. L.R. Squire, N.M. Weinberger, G. Lynch, and J.L. McGaugh. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  31. Eichenbaum, H., Otto, T., and Cohen, N.J. 1992.The hippocampus—what does it do? Behav. Neur. Biol. 57:2–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Eichenbaum, H., Otto, T., and Cohen, N. J. 1994. Two functional components of the hippocampal memory system. Behav. Brain Sci. 17:449–518.Google Scholar
  33. Fanselow, M.S. 1980. Conditioning and unconditional components of post-shock freezing. Pavlovian J. Biol. Sci. 15:177–182.Google Scholar
  34. Fanselow, M.S. 1990. Factors governing one-trial contextual conditioning. Animal Learn. Behav. 18:264–270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Fass, B., Wrege, W., and Greenough, W.T. 1980. Behavioral symptoms following serial or simultaneous septal-forebrain lesions: similar syndromes. Physiol. Behav. 25:683–690.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Flaherty, C.F., Capobianco, S., and Hamilton, L.W. 1979. Septal lesions, sex and incentive shift effects on open field behavior in rats. Physiol. Behav. 22:903–909.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Frankland, P.W., Cestari, V, Filipkowski, R.K., McDonald, R. J., and Silva, A. J. 1998. The dorsal hippocampus is essential for context discrimination but not for contextual conditioning. Behav. Neurosci. 112:863–874.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Fried, P.A. 1972. Septum and behavior: a review. Psychol. Bull. 78:292–310.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Gaffan, D., and Harrison, S. 1989. Place memory and scene memory: effects of fornix transection in the monkey. Exp. Brain Res. 74:202–212.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Gage, F.H., and Olton, D.S. 1975. Hippocampal influences of hyperactivity induced by septal lesions. Brain Res. 98:311–325.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Gage, F.H., Olton, D.S., and Bolanowski, D. 1978. Activity, reactivity, and dominance following septal lesions in rats. Behav. Biol. 22:203–210.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Glick, S.D., Marsanico, R.G., and Greenstein, S. 1974. Differential recovery of function following caudate, hippocampal and septal lesions in mice. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 86:787–792.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Goodlett, C.R., Engellenner, W.J., Burright, R.G., and Donovick, P.J. 1982. Influence of environmental rearing history and postsurgical environmental change on the septal rage syndrome in mice. Physiol. Behav. 28:1077–1081.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Gotsick, J.E. 1969. Factors affecting spontaneous activity in rats with limbic lesions. Physiol. Behav. 4:587–593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Grastyan, E., Karmos, G, Vereczkey, L., and Kellenyi, L. 1966. The hippocampal electrical correlates of the homeostatic regulation of motivation. Electroen-cephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 21:34–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Gray, D.S., Terlecki, L.J., Treit, D., and Pinel, J.P. J. 1981. Effect of septal lesions on conditioned defensive burying. Physiol. Behav. 27:1051–1056.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Gray, J.A. 1982. The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry into the Functions of the Septo-Hippocampal System. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  48. Gray, J.A., and McNaughton, N. 1983. Comparison between the behavioral effects Of septal and hippocampal lesions: a review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 7:119–188.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Gray, J.A., and Rawlins, J.N.P 1986. Comparator and buffer memory: an attempt to integrate two models of hippocampal function. In The Hippocampus, vol. 4, eds. R.L. Issacson and K.H. Pribram, pp. 159–202. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Green, J.D., and Arduini, A. 1954. Hippocampal electrical activity in arousal. J. Neurophysiol. 17:533–557.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Greenstein, Y. J., Pavlides, C., and Winson, J. 1988. Long-term potentiation in the dentate gyrus is preferentially induced at theta rhythm periodicity. Brain Res. 438(l-2):331–334.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Han, J., Gallagher, M., and Holland, P. 1995. Hippocampal lesions disrupt decrements but not increments in conditioned stimulus processing. J. Neurosci. 15: 7323–7329.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Harvey, J.A., Lints, C.E., Jacobson, L.E., and Hunt, H.F. 1965. Effects of lesions in the septal area on conditioned fear and discriminated instrumental punishment in the albino rat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 59:37–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Hedges, A.S., Van Atta, L., and Thomas, J.B. 1975. Septal lesions facilitate the shift from conditioned escape to conditioned avoidance behavior in the rat. Physiol. Behav. 14:25–30.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Hirsh, R. 1974. The hippocampus and contextual retrieval of information from memory: a theory. Behav. Biol. 12:421–444.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Hogg, S. 1996. A review of the validity and variability of the elevated plus-maze as an animal model of anxiety. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 54:21–30.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Honey, R.C., and Good, M. 1993. Selective hippocampal lesions abolish the contextual specificity of latent inhibition and conditioning. Behav. Neurosci. 107:23–33.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Hu, D., and Amsel, A. 1995. A simple test of the vicareous trial and error hypothesis of hippocampal function. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA. 92:5506–5509.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Issacson, R.L. 1982. The Limbic System. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  60. Jarrard, L.E. 1973. The hippocampus and motivation. Psych. Bull. 79:1–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Jarrard, L.E. 1993. Review: on the role of the hippocampus in learning and memory in the rat. Behav. Neural Biol. 60:9–26.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Kaada, B.R. 1951. Somatomotor, autonomic and electrocorticographic responses to electrical stimulation of the “Rhinencephalic” and other structures in primates, cats and dogs. Acta Physiol. Scand. 24 (Suppl. 83):l–258.Google Scholar
  63. Kapp, B.S., Whalen, P. J., Supple, W.F., and Pascoe, J.P 1992. Amygdaloid contributions to conditioned fear. In The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction., ed. J.P. Aggleton, pp. 229–254. New York: Wiley-Liss.Google Scholar
  64. Kenyon, J., and Krieckhaus, E.E., 1965a. Decrements in one-way avoidance learning following septal lesions in rat. Psychonomic Sci. 3:113–114.Google Scholar
  65. Kenyon, J., and Krieckhaus, E.E. 1965b. Decrements in one way avoidance behavior following septal lesions in the rat as a function of lesion size and spontaneous activity. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 59:466–469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Kim, J.J., and Fanselow, M.S. 1992. Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of fear. Science. 256:675–677.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Kimble, D.P 1968. Hippocampus and internal inhibition. Psych. Bull. 68:285–295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. King, F.A. 1958. Effects of septal and amygdala lesions on emotional behavior and conditioned avoidance responses in the rat. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 126:57–63.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. King, F.A., and Meyer, P.M. 1958. Effects of amygdaloid lesions upon septal hyper-emotionality. Science. 128:655–656.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Kohler, C., and Srebro, B. 1980. Effects of lateral and medial septal lesions on exploratory behavior in the albino rat. Brain Res. 182:423–440.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Krieckhaus, E.E., Miller, N.E., and Zimmerman, P. 1965. Reduction of freezing behavior and improvement of shock avoidance by d-amphetamine. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 60:36–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Krieckhaus, E.E., Simmons, HJ., Thomas, G.J., and Kenyon, J. 1964. Septal lesions enhance shock avoidance behavior in the rat. Exp. Neurol. 9:107–113.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Lau, P., and Miczek, K.A. 1977. Differential effects of septal lesions on attack and defensive-submissive reactions during intraspecies aggression in rats. Physiol Behav. 18:479–485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. LeDoux, J.E. 1987. Emotion. In Handbook of Physiology, 1: The Nervous System, Vol V, Higher Functions of the Brain., ed. F. Plum, pp. 419–460. Bethesda, MD: American Physiological Society.Google Scholar
  75. LeDoux, J.E. 1992. Emotion and the amygdala. In The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction., ed. J.P. Aggleton, pp. 339–351. New York: Wiley-Liss.Google Scholar
  76. LeDoux, J.E. 1995. Emotion: clues from the brain. Ann. Rev. Psych. 46:209–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. LeDoux, J.E. 1996. Emotional networks and motor control: a fearful view. In Progress in Brain Research., eds. G. Holstege, R. Bandler, and C.B. Saper, pp. 437–446. New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  78. Lubar, J.E, and Numan, R. 1973. Behavioral and physiological studies of septal function and related medial cortical structures. Behav. Biol. 8:1–25.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Lubow, R.E. 1973. Latent inhibition. Psych. Bull. 79:398–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Lynch, G. 1986. Synapses, Circuits and the Beginnings of Memory. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  81. Lynch, G., Kessler, M., Arai, A., and Larson, J. 1990. The nature and causes of hip-pocampal long-term potentiation. Prog. Brain Res. 83:233–250.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Malick, J.B. 1970. A behavioral comparison of three lesion-induced models of aggression in rat. Physiol. Behav. 5:679–681.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Maren, S., and Fanselow, M.S. 1997. Electrolytic lesions of the fimbria/fornix, dorsal hippocampus, or entorhinal cortex produce anterograde deficits in contextual fear conditioning in rats. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 67:142–149.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Mattingly, B.A., Osborne, F.H., and Gotsick, J.E. 1979. Activity changes during a conditioned aversive stimulus in rats with septal lesions. Physiol Behav. 22:521–525.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. McAllister, W.R., and McAllister, D.E. 1971. Behavioral measurement of conditioned fear. In Aversive Conditioning and Learning., ed. F.R. Brush. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  86. McAlonan, G.M., Wilkinson, L.S., Robbins, T.W., and Everitt, B. J. 1995. The effects of AMPA-induced lesions of the septo-hippocampal cholinergic projections on aversive conditioning to explicit and contextual cues and spatial learning in the water maze. Eur. J. Neurosci. 7:281–292.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. McNaughton, B.L., and Barnes, C.A. 1990. From cooperativc synaptic enhancement to associative memory: bridging the abyss. Sem. Neurosci. 2:403–416.Google Scholar
  88. Melia, K.R., and Davis, M. 1991. Effects of septal lesions on fear-potentiated startle, and on the anxiolytic effects of buspirone and diazepam. Physiol Behav. 49: 603–611.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Melia, K.R., Sananes, C.B., and Davis, M. 1991. Lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala block the excitatory effects of the septal ablation on the acoustic startle reflex. Physiol Behav. 51:175–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Miller, R. 1991. Cortico-Hippocampal Interplay and the Representation of Contexts in the Brain. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  91. Molino, A. 1975. Sparing of function after infant lesions of selected limbic structures in the rat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 89:868–881.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Morgan, M.A., Romanski, L.M., and LeDoux, IE. 1993. Extinction of emotional learning: contribution of medial prefrontal cortex. Neurosci. Lett. 163: 109–113.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Morris, R.G.M., Garrard, P., Rawlins, J.N.P., and O’Keefe, J. 1982. Place Navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature. 273:297–298.Google Scholar
  94. Nadel, L., and Willner, J. 1980. Context and conditioning: a place for space. Physiol. Psych. 8:218–228.Google Scholar
  95. O’Keefe, J., and Nadel, L. 1978. The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  96. Olton, D.S. 1973. Shock-motivated avoidance and the analysis of behavior. Psych. Bull. 79:243–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Olton, D.S., Becker, J.T., and Handelmann, G.E. 1979. Hippocampus, space, and memory. Behav. Brain Sci. 2:313–365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Olton, D.S., and Gage, F.H. 1974. Role of the fornix in septal syndrome. Physiol. Behav. 13:269–279.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Palvides, C, Greenstein, Y.J., Grudman, M., and Winson, J. 1988. Long-term poten-tiation in the dentate gyrus is induced preferentially on the positive phase of the theta-rhythm. Brain Res. 439:383–387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Peacock, E.J., and Wong, P.T.P. 1982. Defensive burying in the rat: a behavioral field analysis. Animal Learn. Behav. 10:103–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Pellow, S.P., Chopin, P., File, S.E., and Briley, M. 1985. Validation of openxlosed arm entries in an elevated plus-maze as a measure of anxiety in the rat. J. Neurosci. Meth. 14:149–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Pellow, S., and File, S.E. 1986. Anxiolytic and anxiogenic drug effects on exploratory activity in an elevated plus-maze: a novel test of anxiety in a rat. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 24:525–529.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Penick, S., and Solomon, PR. 1991. Hippocampus, context, and conditioning. Behav. Neurosci. 105:611–617.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Pesold, C., and Treit, D. 1992. Excitotoxic lesions of the septum produce anxiolytic effects in the elevated plus-maze and the shock-probe burying tests. Physiol. Behav. 52:37–47.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Pesold, C., and Treit, D. 1994. The septum and amygdala differentially mediate the anxiolytic effects of benzodiazepines. Brain Res. 638:295–301.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Pesold, C., and Treit, D. 1995. The central and basolateral amygdala differentially mediate the anxiolytic effects of benzodiazepines. Brain Res. 671:213–221.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Phillips, R.G., and LeDoux, J.E. 1992. Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behav. Neurosci. 106: 274–285.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Phillips, R.G., and LeDoux, J.E. 1994. Lesions of the dorsal hippocampal formation interfere with background but not foreground contextual fear conditioning. Learn. Mem. 1:33–44.Google Scholar
  109. Pinel, J.P.J., and Treit, D. 1978. Burying as a defensive response in rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 92:708–712.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  110. Pinel, J.P.J., and Treit, D. 1979. Conditioned defensive burying in rats: availability of burying material. Animal Learn. Behav. 12:392–396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Poplawsky, A., and Isaacson, R.L. 1983. Changes in open-field behaviors following septal lesions in rats. Behav. Neural Biol. 38:61–69.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  112. Powell, D.A., Milligan, W.L., and Mull, P. 1982. Lateral septal lesions enhance conditioned bradycardia in the rabbit. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 96:742–754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  113. Rawlins, J.N.P. 1985. Associations across time: the hippocampus as a temporary memory store. Behav. Brain Sci. 8:479–496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  114. Rescorla, R.A., and Wagner, A.R. 1972. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory., eds. A.H. Black, and W.F. Prokasy, pp. 64–99. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  115. Rodgers, R. J., and Johnson, J.T. 1995. Factor analysis of spatiotemporal and etho-logical measures in the murine elevated plus-maze test of anxiety. Pharm. Biochem. Behav. 52:297–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  116. Rudy, J.W., and Sutherland, R. J. 1989. The hippocampal formation is necessary for rats to learn and remember configurai discriminations. Behav. Brain Res. 34:97–109.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  117. Rudy, J.W., and Sutherland, R.J. 1992. Configurai and elemental associations and the memory coherence problem. J. Cog. Neurosci. 4:208–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  118. Rudy, J.W., and Sutherland, R. J. 1995. Configurai association theory and the hippocampal formation: An appraisal and reconfiguration. Hippocampus. 5:375–389.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  119. Sagvolden, T. 1975a. Operant responding for water in rats with septal lesions: effects of deprivation level. Behav. Biol. 13:323–330.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  120. Sagvolden, T. 1975b. Acquisition of two-way active avoidance following septal lesions in the rat: effect of intensity of discontinuous shock. Behav. Biol. 14:59–74.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  121. Sagvolden, T. 1976. Free-operant avoidance behavior in rats with lateral septal lesions: effect of shock intensity. Brain Res. 110:559–574.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  122. Sagvolden, T. 1979. Behavior of rats with septal lesions during low levels of water deprivation. Behav. Neural Biol. 26:431–441.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  123. Schmajuk, N.A., Lam, Y., and Christiansen, B.A. 1994. Latent inhibition of the rat eyeblink response: Effect of hippocampal aspiration lesions. Physiol. Behav. 55:597–601.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  124. Schwartzbaum, J.S., and Gay, P.E. 1966. Interacting behavioral effects of septal and amygdaloid lesions in rat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 61:59–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  125. Seldon, N.R.W., Everitt, B. J., Jarrard, L.E., and Robbins, T.W. 1991. Complementary roles for the amygdala and hippocampus in aversive conditioning to explicit and contextual cues. Neuroscience. 42:335–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  126. Singh, D. 1969. Comparison of hyperemotionality caused by lesions in the septal and ventromedial hypothalamic areas in the rat. Psychonomic Sci. 16:3–4.Google Scholar
  127. Singh, D. 1973. Comparison of behavioral deficits caused by lesions in septal and ventromedial hypothalamic area. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 84:370–379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  128. Slotnick, B.M., and Brown, D.L. 1970. Fear conditioning and passive avoidance in mice with septal lesions. Physiol. Behav. 5:1255–1259.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  129. Slotnick, B.M., and Jarvick, M.E. 1966. Deficits in passive avoidance and fear conditioning in mice with septal lesions. Science. 154:1207–1208.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  130. Solomon, P.R., and Moore, J.W. 1975. Latent inhibition and stimulus generalization of the classically conditioned nictitating membrane response in rabbits following dorsal hippocampal ablation. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 89:1192–1203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  131. Sparks, P.D. 1998. The Role of the Septal Complex in the Modulation of Emotion. Doctoral dissertation, New York University, New York.Google Scholar
  132. Sparks, P.D., and LeDoux, J.E. 1995. Septal lesions potentiate freezing behavior to contextual but not to phasic conditioned stimuli in rats. Behav. Neurosci. 109:184–188.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  133. Spiegel, E.A., Miller, H.R., and Oppenheimer, M. J. 1940. Forebrain and rage reactions. J. Neurophysiol. 3:538–548.Google Scholar
  134. Squire, L.R. 1987. Memory and Brain. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  135. Squire, L.R. 1992. Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis of findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psych. Rev. 99:195–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  136. Staubli, U.V. 1994. Parallel properties of LTP and Memory. In Brain and Memory: Modulation and Mediation of Neuroplasticity., ed. J.L. McGaugh. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  137. Staubli, U.V. 1995. Parallel properties of long term potentiation and memory. In Brain and Memory: Modulation and Mediation of Neuroplasticity., eds. J.L. McGaugh, N.M. Weinberger, and G. Lynch, pp. 303–318. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  138. Staubli, U.V., and Lynch, G. 1990. Stable depression of potentiated synaptic responses in the hippocampus with 1-5 Hz stimulation. Brain Res. 513:113–118.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  139. Sutherland, R.X, McDonald, R. J., Hill, C.R., and Rudy, J.W. 1989. Damage to the hippocampal formation in rats selectively impairs the ability to learn cue relationships. Behav. Neural Biol. 52:331–356.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  140. Sutherland, R.J., and Rudy, J.W. 1989. Configurai association theory: The role of the hippocampal formation in learning, memory, and amnesia. Psychobiology. 17:129–144.Google Scholar
  141. Swanson, L.W., Kohler, C., and Bjorklund, A. 1987. The limbic region. I: The septo-hippocampal system. In Handbook of Chemical Neuroanatomy, Vol. 5: Integrated Systems in the Brain., eds.A. Bjorklund, T. Hokfelt, and L.W. Swanson. B.V.: Elsevier Science Publishers.Google Scholar
  142. Theios, J., Lynch, A.D., and Lowe W.F., Jr. 1966. Differential effects of shock intensity on one-way and shuttle avoidance conditioning. J. Exp. Psych. 72:294–349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  143. Thomas, E. 1988. Forebrain mechanisms in the relief of fear: the role of the lateral septum. Psychobiology. 16:36–44.Google Scholar
  144. Thomas, E., and Evans, G.J. 1983. Septal inhibition of aversive emotional states. Physiol. Behav. 31:673–678.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  145. Thomas, J.B. 1973. Some behavioral effects of olfactory bulb damage in the rat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 83:140–148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  146. Thomas, J.B., and McCleary, R.A. 1974. One way avoidance behavior and septal lesions in rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 86:751–759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  147. Tolman, E.C. 1938. The determiners of behavior at a choice point. Psych. Rev. 45:1–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  148. Trafton, C.L. 1967. Effects of lesions in the septal area and cingulate cortical areas on conditioned suppression of activity and avoidance behavior in rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 63:191–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  149. Treit, D. 1990. A comparison of anxiolytic and nonanxiolytic agents in the shock-probe/burying test for anxiolytics. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 36:203–205.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  150. Treit, D., Harinder, A., and Menard, J. 1998. Does the bed nucleus of the stria ter-minalis mediate fear behaviors? Behav. Neurosci. 112:379–386.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  151. Treit, D., Lolordo, V.M., and Armstrong, D.E. 1986.The effects of diazepam on “fear” reactions in rats are modulated by environmental constraints on the rat’s defensive repertoire. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 25:561–565PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  152. Treit, D., and Pesold, C. 1990. Septal lesions inhibit fear reactions in two animal models of anxiolytic drug action. Physiol. Behav. 47:365–371.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  153. Treit, D., Pesold, C., and Rotzinger, S. 1993. Dissociating the anti-fear effects of septal and amygdaloid lesions using two pharmacologically validated models of rat anxiety. Behav. Neurosci. 107:770–785.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  154. Vertes, R.P. 1986. Brainstem modulations of the hippocampus: anatomy, physiology, and significance. In The Hippocampus, Vol 4., eds. R.L. Isaacson, and K.H. Pribram, pp. 41–68. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  155. Wagman, R. 1972. Septal Area Modulation of Conditioned Emotional States in Cats. Unpublished masters thesis, Bryn Mawr College, Bryn Mawr, PA.Google Scholar
  156. Whimby, A.E., and Denenberg, V.H. 1967. Two independent behavioral dimensions in open field performance. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 63:500–504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  157. Whishaw, I.Q. 1998. Spatial mapping takes time. Hippocampus. 8:122–130.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  158. Winkelgren, W.A. 1979. Chunking and consolidation: A theoretical synthesis of semantic networks configuring in conditioning, S-R versus cognitive learning, normal forgetting, the amnesic syndrome, and the hippocampal arousal system. Psych. Rev. 86:44–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  159. Woodruff, M.L., and Lippoincott, W.I. 1976. Hyperemotionality and enhanced tonic immobility after septal lesions in the rabbit. Brain Behav. Evol. 13:22–33.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  160. Yee, B.K., Feldon, J., and Rawlins, J.N.P. 1995. Latent inhibition in rats is abolished by NMDA-induced neuronal loss in the retrohippocampal region, but this lesion effect can be prevented by systemic haloperidol treatment. Behav. Neurosci. 109:227–240.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  161. Zucker, I. 1965. Effect of lesions of the septal-limbic area on the behavior of cats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 60:344–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  162. Zuromski, E.S., Donovick, P. J., and Burright, R.G. 1972. Bar pressing for illumination change in albino rats with septal lesions. J. Comp. Physiol Psych. 78:83–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2000

Authors and Affiliations

  • Peter D. Sparks
  • Joseph E. LeDoux

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations