Advertisement

Subjective Referral of the Timing for a Conscious Sensory Experience

A Functional Role for The Somatosensory Specific Projection System in Man
  • Benjamin Libet
  • Elwood W. WrightJr.
  • Bertram Feinstein
  • Dennis K. Pearl
Chapter
Part of the Contemporary Neuroscientists book series (CN)

Abstract

PREVIOUS studies had indicated that there is a substantial delay, up to about 0.5 s, before activity at cerebral levels achieves ‘neuronal adequacy’ for eliciting a conscious somatosensory experience (Libet, Alberts, Wright, Delattre, Levin and Feinstein, 1964; Libet, 1966). The delay appeared necessary not only with stimulation of medial lemniscus, ventrobasal thalamus, or postcentral cortex, but even when the stimulus was a single electrical pulse at the skin (Libet, Alberts, Wright, and Feinstein, 1967, 1972; Libet, 1973). The present investigation began with an experimental test of whether there is in fact also a subjective delay in the conscious experience for a peripheral sensory stimulus. That is, is there a delay in the subjective timing of the experience that would correspond to the presumed delay in achieving the neuronal state that ‘produces’ the experience? The results of that test led to a modified hypothesis ; this postulates (a) the existence of a subjective referral of the timing for a sensory experience, and (b) a role for the specific (lemniscal) projection system in mediating such a subjective referral of timing. Experimental tests of the new proposal were carried out and are reported here.

Keywords

Sensory Experience Conscious Experience Peripheral Stimulus Stimulus Train Subjective Timing 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Azulay, A. and Schartz, A. S. (1975) The role of the dorsal funiculus of the primate in tactile discrimination. Experimental Neurology, 46, 315–332}.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bartlett, J. R., Doty, R. W., Lee, B. B., Negrão, N. and Overman, W. H. (1977) Deleterious effects of prolonged electrical excitation of striate cortex in Macaques. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 14, 46–66.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Brain, Lord. (1963) Some reflections on brain and mind. Brain, 86, 381–402.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Colonnier, M. L. (1966) The structural design of the neocortex. In: Brain and Conscious Experience. Edited by J. C. Eccles. New York: Springer-Verlag, pp. 1–23.Google Scholar
  5. Delgado, J. M. R. (1955) Evaluation of permanent implantation of electrodes within the brain. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 7, 637–644.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Dempster, A. P., Laird, N. M. and Rubin, D. B. (1977) Maximum likelihood from incomplete data via the EM algorithm. Royal Statistical Society Journal, B, 39, 1–38.Google Scholar
  7. Desmedt, J. E. (1971) Somatosensory cerebral evoked potentials in man. In: Handbook of Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. Edited by A. Rémond. Amsterdam: Elsevier, Vol. 9, pp. 8–82.Google Scholar
  8. Desmedt, J. E. and Robertson, D. (1977) Differential enhancement of early and late components of the cerebral somatosensory evoked potentials during forced-paced cognitive tasks in man. Journal of Physiology, London, 271, 761–782.Google Scholar
  9. Feinstein, B. F., Alberts, W. W. and Levin, G. (1969) The use of implanted thermistor electrodes in the therapy of parkinsonism. In: Proceedings of the Third Symposium on Parkinson’s Disease. Edited by F. J. Gillingham and I. M. L. Donaldson. Edinburgh: Livingstone, pp. 232–237.Google Scholar
  10. Wright, E. W., Jr. and Levin, G. (1960) A stereotaxic technique in man allowing multiple spatial and temporal approaches to intracranial targets. Journal of Neurosurgery, 17, 708–720.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Goff, G. D., Matsumiya, Y., Allison, T. and Goff, W. R. (1977) The scalp topography of human somatosensory and auditory evoked potentials. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 42, 57–76.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Hern, J. E. C., Landgren, S., Phillips, C. G. and Porter, R. (1962) Selective excitation of corticofugal neurons by surface-anodal stimulation of the baboon’s motor cortex. Journal of Physiology, London, 161, 73–90.Google Scholar
  13. Hirsch, J. F., Pertuiset, B., Calvet, J., Buisson-ferey, J., Fischgold, H. and Scherrer, J. (1961) Etude des réponses électrocorticales obtenues chez l’homme par des stimulations somesthésiques et visuelles. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 13, 411–424.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Jasper, H., Lende, R. and Rasmussen, T. (1960) Evoked potentials from the exposed somatosensory cortex in man. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 130, 526–537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Libet, B. (1965) Cortical activation in conscious and unconscious experience. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 9, 77–86.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Libet, B. (1966) Brain stimulation and the threshold of conscious experience. In: Brain and Conscious Experience. Edited by J. Eccles. New York: Springer-Verlag, pp. 165–181.Google Scholar
  17. Libet, B. (1973) Electrical stimulation of cortex in human subjects and conscious sensory aspects. In: Handbook of Sensory Physiology. Edited by A. Iggo. Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag, Vol. 2, pp. 743–790.Google Scholar
  18. Libet, B. (1978) Neuronal vs. subjective timing, for a conscious sensory experience. In: Cerebral Correlates of Conscious Experience. Edited by P. Buser and A. Rougeul-Buser}. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 69–82.Google Scholar
  19. Libet, B. Alberts, W. W., Wright, E. W., Jr., Delattre, L. D., Levin, G. and Feinstein, B. (1964) Production of threshold levels of conscious sensation by electrical stimulation of human somatosensory cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 27, 546–578.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Libet, B., Alberts, W. W., Wright, E. W. and Feinstein, B. (1967) Responses of human somatosensory cortex to stimuli below threshold for conscious sensation. Science, 158,1597–1600.Google Scholar
  21. Libet, B., Alberts, W. W., Wright, E. W., Feinstein, B. (1972) Cortical and thalamic activation in conscious sensory experience. In: Neurophysiology Studied in Man. Edited by G. G. Somjen. Amsterdam: Excerpta Medica, pp.157–168.Google Scholar
  22. Libet, B., Alberts, W. W., Wright, E. W., Lewis, M. and Feinstein, B. (1975) Cortical representation of evoked potentials relative to conscious sensory responses, and of somatosensory qualities in man. In: The Somatosensory System. Edited by H. H. Kornhuber. Stuttgart: Georg Thieme, pp. 291–308.Google Scholar
  23. Nashold, B., Somjen, G. and Friedman, H. (1972) Parasthesias and EEG potentials evoked by stimulation of the dorsal funiculi in man. Experimental Neurology, 36, 273–287.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Phillips, C. G. (1969) Motor apparatus of the baboon’s hand. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B, 173, 141–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Popper, K. R. and Eccles, J. C. (1977) The Self and Its Brain. Berlin: Springer, pp. 256–259, 362-365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Pudenz, R. H., Bullara, L. A. and Talalla, A. (1975) Electrical stimulation of the brain. Surgical Neurology, 4, 265–270, 389-400.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Sternberg, S. and Knoll, R. L. (1973) The perception of temporal order: fundamental issues and a general model. In: Attention and Performance. Edited by S. Kornblum. New York: Academic Press, No. 4, pp. 629–685.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1993

Authors and Affiliations

  • Benjamin Libet
  • Elwood W. WrightJr.
  • Bertram Feinstein
    • 1
  • Dennis K. Pearl
    • 1
  1. 1.the Neurological Institute, Department of NeuroscienceMount Zion HospitalSan FranciscoUSA

Personalised recommendations