The Neural Time — Factor in Perception, Volition and Free Will

  • Benjamin Libet
Chapter
Part of the Contemporary Neuroscientists book series (CN)

Abstract

We should all agree that the brain and the mind are intimately linked. One need only drink some wine to remind one of the physicochemical controls of mental function, acting via the brain. And one need merely decide at will to flick a finger, or not to flick it, at any time one wishes to do so, to demonstrate thet a mental event can apparently « order » the brain to produce an action. Yes this kind of prima facie evidence of a mutually causative interaction between mental and cerebral processes has been subjected to an immense amount of philosophical analyses and arguments over its « true » basis. These ranged from materialist views of conscious mental functions as meaningless epiphenomena to dualist, spiritualistic views of a separable mental entity with an ability to control neural function. Althought a causative role for conscious mental processes is readily accommodated within a dualist interactionist theory of mind and brain (e.g. Popper and Eccles, 1978) it has also been argued within the framework of a monist determinist theory (e.g. Sperry, 1980).

Keywords

Lime Peri Dura Stim Exter 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

RÉFÉRences

  1. Adrian, E.D. (1952), « What happens when we think », in The Physical Basis of Mind, ed. Laslett, P., Oxford, Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
  2. Churchland, P.S. (1981), « On the alleged backwards referral of experiences and its relevance to the mind-body problem », Philosophy of Science, 48, p. 165–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Deecke, L., Grozinger, B. et Kornhuber, H.H. (1976), « Voluntary finger movement in man / Cerebral potentials and theory », Biological Cybernetics, 23, p. 99–119.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Kornhuber, H.H. et Deecke, L. (1965), « Hirnpotentialä nderungen bei Willkürbewegungen und passiven Bewegungen des Menschen: Bereitschaftspotential und reafferente Potentiale », Pflugers Archiv fur Gesamte Physiologie, 284, p. 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Libet, B. (1965), « Cortical activation in conscious and unconscious experience », Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 9, p. 77–86.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Libet, B. (1966), « Brain stimulation and the threshold of conscious experience », in Brain and conscious experience, J.C. Eccles (ed.), Springer, p. 168–181.Google Scholar
  7. Libet, B. (1973), « Electrical stimulation of cortex in human subjects, and conscious sensory aspects », in Handbook of sensory physiology, A. Iggo (ed.), Springer, p. 743–790.Google Scholar
  8. Libet, B. (1978), « Neuronal vs. subjective timing for a conscious sensory experience », in Cerebral correlates of conscious experience, P.A. Buser et A. Rougeul-Buser (ed.), Elsevier/North Holland Biomé dical Press, p. 69–82.Google Scholar
  9. Libet, B. (1981), « The experimental evidence for subjective referral of a sensory experience backwards in time », Philosophy of Science, 48, p. 182–197CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Libet, B. (1982), « Brain stimulation in the study of neuronal functions for conscious sensory experiences », Human Neurobiology, 1, 235–242.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Libet, B. (1985), « Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action », Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, p. 529–566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Lebet, B. (1987), « Consciousness: Conscious, Subjective Experience », in Encyclopedia of Neuroscience, G. Adelman (ed.), Birkhauser, Boston, p. 271–275.Google Scholar
  13. Libet, B. (1989), « Conscious subjective experience vs. unconscious mental functions: A theory of the cerebral processes involved », in Models of brain function, R.M.J. Cotterill (ed.), Cambridge U. Press, p. 35–49.Google Scholar
  14. Libet, B., Gleason, CA., Wright, E.W. et Pearl, D.K. (1983), « Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activities (readiness-potential); the unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act », Brain, 106, p. 623–642.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Libet, B., Pearl, D.K., Morledge, D.M., Gleason, CA., Hosobuchi, Y. et Barbaro, N.M. (1991), « Control of the transition from sensory detection to sensory awareness in man by duration of a thalamic stimulus », Brain, 114, p. 1731–1757.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Libet, B., Wright, E.W., Jr., Feinstein, B. et Pearl, D.K. (1979), « Subjective referral of the timing for a conscious sensory experience: A functional role for the somatosensory specific projection system in man », Brain, 102, p. 193–224.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Nagel, T. (1979), Mortal questions, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  18. Penfield, W. (1958), The excitable cortex in conscious man, Liverpool University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Pepper, S.C. (1960), « A neural — identity theory of mind », in Dimensions of Mind, S. Hook (ed.), New York University Press, Washington Square, p. 37–56.Google Scholar
  20. Popper, K.R. et Eccles, J.C. (1977), The self and its brain, Springer.Google Scholar
  21. Sperry, R.W. (1980), « Mind-Brain interaction: Mentalism, yes; dualism, no », Neuroscience, 5, p. 195–206.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Taylor, J.L. et Mccloskey, D.I. (1990), « Triggering of preprogrammed movements as reactions to masked stimuli », J. Neurophysiology, 63, p. 439–446.Google Scholar

References

  1. Burns BD (1951): Some properties of isolated cerebral cortex in the unanesthetized cat. J Physiol (Lond.) 112:156–175Google Scholar
  2. Burns BD (1954): The production of after-bursts in isolated unanesthetized cerebral cortex. J Physiol (Lond.) 125:427–446Google Scholar
  3. Doty RW (1984): Some thoughts and some experiments on memory. In: Neuropsychology of Memory, LR Squire and N Butters, eds., New York: uilford, pp. 330–339Google Scholar
  4. Doty RW (1985): The time course of conscious processing: Vetoes by the uninformed? Behavioral and Brain Sci 8:541–542CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Eccles JC (1990): A unitary hypothesis of mind-brain interaction in cerebral cortex. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 240:433–451CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Echlin FA, Arnett V and Zoll J (1952): Paroxysmal high voltage discharges from isolated and partially isolated human and animal cerebral cortex. Electroenceph & Clin Neurophysiol 4:147–164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Garcia Ramos J and Ibarra BH (1973): Studies on the mechanisms of learning, II, on the ionic nature of the dendritic action potentials and mescaline spikes. Ada Physiol Latino Amer 23:202–212Google Scholar
  8. Goldring S, O’Leary JL, Holmes TG and Jerva MJ (1961): Direct response of isolated cerebral cortex of cat. J Neurophysiol 24:633–650PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Gray CM and Singer W (1989): Stimulus-specific neuronal oscillations in orientation columns of cat visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 86:1698–1702PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Henry CE and Scoville WB (1952): Suppression-burst activity from isolated cerebral cortex in man. Electroenceph & Clin Neurophysiol 4:1–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Holender D (1986): Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: A survey and appraisal. Behavioral and Brain Sci 9:1–66CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Ingvar D (1955a): Electrical activity of isolated cortex in the unanesthetized cat with intact brain stem. Acta Physiol Scand 33:151–168PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Ingvar D (1955b): Extraneuronal influences upon the electrical activity of isolated cortex following stimulation of the rettricular activating system. Ada Physiol Scand 33:169–193CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Ingvar D and Phillipson L (1977): Distibutions of cerebral blood flow in the dominant hemisphere during motor ideation and motor performance. Ann Neurol 2:230–237PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Jensen AR (1979): “ g”: Outmoded theory or unconquered frontier? Creative Science and Technology 11:16–29Google Scholar
  16. Kristiansen K and Courtois G (1949): Rhythmic electrical activity from isolated cerebral cortex. Electroenceph & Clin Neurophysiol 1:265–272Google Scholar
  17. Libet B (1965) [see this book, no. 2]Google Scholar
  18. Libet B et al. (1972) [see this book, no. 7]Google Scholar
  19. Libet B (1973) [see this book, no. 5]Google Scholar
  20. Libet B (1978) [see this book, no. 8]Google Scholar
  21. Libet B (1982) [see this book, no. 12]Google Scholar
  22. Libet B (1985) [see this book, no. 16]Google Scholar
  23. Libet B (1987) [see this book, no. 18]Google Scholar
  24. Libet B (1989) [see this book, no. 20]Google Scholar
  25. Libet B, Alberts WW, Wright EW, De Lattre LD, Levin G and Feinstein B (1964) [see this book, no. 1]Google Scholar
  26. Libet B, Wright EW and Gleason C (1982) [see this book, no. 13]Google Scholar
  27. Libet B, Wright EW and Gleason C (1983a) [see this book, no. 14]Google Scholar
  28. Libet B, Wright EW, Gleason CA and Pearl DK (1983b) [see this book, no. 15]Google Scholar
  29. Libet B, Wright EW, Feinstein B and Pearl DK (1979) [see this book, no. 9]Google Scholar
  30. Libet B, Pearl DK, Morledge DM, Gleason CA, Hosobuchi Y and Barbara NM (1991) [see this book, no. 21]Google Scholar
  31. Libet B, Wright EW, Feinstein B and Pearl DK (1992) [see this book, no. 10]Google Scholar
  32. McKay DM (1985): Do we control our brains? Behavioral and Brain Sci 8:546CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Penfield W (1958): The Excitable Cortex in Conscious Man. Liverpool: Liverpool University PressGoogle Scholar
  34. Popper KR and Eccles JC (1977): The Self and its Brain. Heidelberg: SpringerCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Rech RH and Domino EF (1960): Effects of various drugs on activity of the neuronally isolated cerebral cortex. Exper Neurol 2:364–378CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Roland PE and Friberg L (1985): Localization of cortical areas activated by thinking. J Neurophysiol 53:1219–1243PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Sherrington CS (1940): Man in His Nature. Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
  38. Singer W (1991): Response synchronization of cortical neurons: an epiphenomenon or a solution to the binding problem. IBRO News 19:6–7 (New York: Pergamon)Google Scholar
  39. Sperry RW (1947): Cerebral regulation of motor coordination in monkeys following multiple transactions of sensorimotor cortex. J Neurophysiol 10:275–294PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Sperry RW (1952): Neurology and the mind-brain problem. American Scientist 40:291–312Google Scholar
  41. Sperry RW (1980): Mind-brain interaction: Mentalism yes; dualism no. Neuroscience 5:195–206PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Sperry RW (1985): Science and Moral Priority. Westport, Conn.: raeger PublGoogle Scholar
  43. Sperry RW, Gazzaniga MS and Bogen JE (1969): Interhemispheric relationships: The neocortical commissures: Syndromes of hemisphere disconnection. In: Handbook of Clinical Neurology vol. 4, PJ Vinken and GW Bruyn (eds), Amsterdam: North Holland PublGoogle Scholar
  44. Taylor JL and McCloskey DI (1990): Triggering of preprogrammed movements as reactions to masked stimuli. J Neurophysiol 63:439–446PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Weiskrantz L (1986): Blindsight: A Case Study and Implications. Oxford: Clarendon PressGoogle Scholar
  46. Wood CC (1985): Pardon, your dualism is showing. Behavioral and Brain Sci 8:557–558CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1993

Authors and Affiliations

  • Benjamin Libet

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations