Advertisement

Thirst pp 279-295 | Cite as

Learning, Thirst and Drinking

  • P. C. Holland
Part of the ILSI Human Nutrition Reviews book series (ILSI HUMAN)

Abstract

Animals’ ingestion of fluids is known to be affected by many factors, some involving events of the external environment, some involving detection of internal changes, some relating to immediate fluid needs, and some not related to fluid deficits. Many of the mechanisms used to detect, integrate, and act on these kinds of information are modifiable by rather specific experience. This chapter concerns a few of these learned influences on thirst and drinking, mostly in rats.

Keywords

Conditioned Stimulus Conditioned Inhibition Pavlovian Conditioning Fluid Deficit Occasion Setting 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Amsel A, Maltzman I (1950) The effect upon generalized drive strength of emotionality as inferred from the level of consummatory response. J Exp Psychol 40:563–569PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Antelman SM, Rowland NE, Fisher AE (1976) Stimulation bound ingestive behavior: a view from the tail. Physiol Behav 17:743–748PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bouton ME, Swartzentruber D (1986) Analysis of the associative and occasion-setting properties of contexts participating in a Pavlovian discrimination. J Exp Psychol [Anim Behav] 12:333–350CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Braveman NS, Jarvis PS (1978) Independence of neophobia and taste aversion learning. Anim Learn Behav 6:406–412CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Burke GH, Mook DG, Blass EM (1972) Hyperreactivity to quinine associated with osmotic thirst in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol 78:32–39PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bykoff KM (1957) The cerebral cortex and the internal organs. Chemical Publishing Co, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  7. Cabanac M (1971) Physiological role of pleasure. Science 173:1103–1107PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Campbell BA (1960) Effects of water deprivation on random activity. J Comp Physiol Psychol 53:240–241PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Davidson TL (1987) Learning about deprivation intensity stimuli. Behav Neurosci 101:198–208PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Davidson TL, Flynn FW, Grill HJ (1988) Comparison of the interoceptive consequences of CCK, LiCl, and satiety in rats. Behav Neurosci 102:134–140PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Debold RC, Miller NE, Jensen DO (1965) Effect of strength of drive determined by a new technique for appetitive classical conditioning of rats. J Comp Physiol Psychol 59:102–108PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dickinson A (1980) Contemporary learning theory. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  13. Domjan M (1977) Attenuation and enhancement of neophobia for edible substances. In: Barker LF, Best MR, Domjan M (eds) Learning mechanisms in food selection. Baylor University Press, Waco, TX, pp 151–179Google Scholar
  14. Epstein AN (1960) Water intake without the act of drinking. Science 131:497–498PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Epstein AN (1973) Epilogue: retrospect and prognosis. In: Epstein AN, Kissileff HR, Stellar E (eds) The neuropsychology of thirst: new findings and advances in concepts. Winston, Washington DC, pp 315–322Google Scholar
  16. Ettinger RH, Thompson S, Staddon JER (1986) Cholecystokinin, lithium chloride and feeding regulation in rats. Physiol Behav 36:801–809PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Falk JL (1964) Studies on schedule-induced polydipsia. In: Wayner MJ (ed) Thirst. Pergamon Press, New York, pp 95–116Google Scholar
  18. Flaherty CF, Grigson PS, Brady A (1987) Relative novelty of conditioning context influences directionality of glycemic conditioning. J Exp Psychol [Anim Behav] 13:144–149CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Fudim OK (1978) Sensory preconditioning of flavors with a formalin-produced sodium need. J Exp Psychol [Anim Behav] 4:276–285CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Garcia J, Lasiter PS, Bermudez-Rattoni F, Deems DA (1985) A general theory of aversion learning. Ann NY Acad Sci 43:8–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Ghent L (1957) Some effects of deprivation on eating and drinking behavior: J Comp Physiol Psychol 50:172–176PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Hall WG, Blass EM (1977) Orogastric determinants of drinking in rats: interaction between absorptive and peripheral controls. J Comp Physiol Psychol 91:365–373PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Holland PC (1983) Occasion-setting in Pavlovian feature positive discriminations. In: Commons ML, Herrnstein RL, Wagner AR (eds) Quantitative analyses of behavior: discrimination processes, vol 4. Ballinger, New York, pp 183–206Google Scholar
  24. Holland PC (1984) The origins of Pavlovian conditioned behavior. In: Bower G (ed) The psychology of learning and motivation, vol 18. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ, pp 129–173Google Scholar
  25. Holland PC (1985) The nature of conditioned inhibition in serial and simultaneous feature negative discriminations. In: Miller RR, Spear NE (eds) Information processing in animals: conditioned inhibition. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 267–297Google Scholar
  26. Holland PC (1988) Excitation and inhibition in unblocking. J Exp Psychol [Anim Behav] 14:261–279CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Holland PC (1989a) Acquisition and transfer of conditional discrimination performance. J Exp Psychol [Anim Behav] 15:154–165CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Holland PC (1989b) Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting. Anim Learn Behav 17:269–279CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Holland PC (1989c) Transfer of negative occasion setting and conditioned inhibition across conditioned and unconditioned stimuli. J Exp Psychol [Anim Behav] 15:311–328CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Holland PC (1990a) Event representation in Pavlovian conditioning: image and action. Cognition 37:105–131PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Holland PC (1990b) Forms of memory in Pavlovian conditioning. In: McGaugh JL, Weinberger NM, Lynch G (eds) Brain organization and memory: cells, systems, and circuits. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  32. Holland PC, Rescorla RA (1975) The effect of two ways of devaluing the unconditioned stimulus after first- and second-order appetitive conditioning. J Exp Psychol [Anim Behav] 1:355–363CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hull CL (1933) Differential habituation to internal stimuli in the albino rat. J Comp Psychol 16:255–273CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hull CL (1934) Learning: II. The factor of the conditioned reflex. In: Murchison C (ed) A handbook of general experimental psychology. Clark University Press, Worcester, MA, pp 382–455CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Hull CL (1945) Discrimination of stimulus configurations and the hypothesis of afferent neural interaction. Psychol Rev 52:133–142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Hunt GL, Smith WJ (1967) Pecking and initial drinking responses in young domestic fowl. J Comp Physiol Psychol 64:230–236PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Jenkins HM, Moore BR (1973) The form of the auto-shaped response with food or water reinforcers. J Exper Anal Behav 20:163–181CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Kamin LJ (1968) Attention-like processes in classical conditioning. In: Jones MR (ed) Miami symposium on the prediction of behavior: aversive stimulation. University of Miami Press, Coral Gables, FL, pp 9–32Google Scholar
  39. Kendler HH (1946) The influence of simultaneous hunger and thirst upon the learning of two opposed spatial responses in the white rat. J Exp Psychol 36:212–220PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Kissileff HR (1973) Nonhomeostatic controls of drinking. In: Epstein AN, Kissileff HR, Stellar E (eds) The neuropsychology of thirst: new findings and advances in concepts. Winston, Washington, DC, pp 163–198Google Scholar
  41. Konorski J (1967) Integrative activity of the brain. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  42. Leeper R (1935) The role of motivation in learning: a study of the phenomenon of differential motivational control of the utilization of habits. J Gen Psychol 46:3–40Google Scholar
  43. Lysle DT, Lyte M, Fowler H, Rabin BS (1987) Shock-induced modulation of lymphocyte reactivity: suppression, habituation, and recovery. Life Sci 41:1805–1814PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Mackintosh NJ (1975) A theory of attention: variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychol Rev 82:276–298CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Mackintosh NJ (1983) Conditioning and associative learning. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  46. Mendelson J, Chillag D (1970) Tongue cooling: a new reward for thirsty rodents. Science 170:1418–1421PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Mook DG (1988) On the organization of satiety. Appetite 11:27–39PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Mook DG, Wagner S (1988) Sham drinking of glucose solution in rats: some effects of hydration. Appetite 10:71–87PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Mook DG, Dreifuss S, Keats PH (1986) Satiety for glucose in the rat: the specificity is postingestive. Physiol Behav 36:897–901PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Mook DG, Wagner S, Schwartz LA (1988) Glucose sham drinking in the rat: satiety for the sweet taste without the sweet taste. Appetite 10:89–102PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Mowrer OH (1960) Learning theory and behavior. Wiley, New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Nicolaïdis S, Rowland N (1974) Long-term self-intravenous ‘drinking’ in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol 87:1–15PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Novin D, Miller NE (1962) Failure to condition thirst induced by feeding dry food to hungry rats. J Comp Physiol Psychol 55:373–374PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Oatley K, Dickinson A (1970) Air drinking and the measurement of thirst. Anim Behav 18:259–265CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Pelchat M, Grill HJ, Rozin P, Jacobs J (1983) Quality of acquired responses to tastes by Rattus norvegicus depends on type of associated discomfort. J Comp Physiol Psychol 97:140–153Google Scholar
  56. Poulos CX, Hinson RE (1984) A homeostatic model of Pavlovian conditioning: tolerance to scopolamine-induced adipsia. J Exp Psychol [Anim Behav] 10:75–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Premack D (1965) Reinforcement theory. In: Levine D (ed) Nebraska symposium on motivation: 1965. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, NEGoogle Scholar
  58. Ramachandran R, Pearce JM (1987) Pavlovian analysis of interactions between hunger and thirst. J Exp Psychol [Anim Behav] 13:182–192CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Rescorla RA (1985) Conditioned inhibition and facilitation. In: Miller RR, Spear NE (eds) Information processing in animals: conditioned inhibition. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 299–326Google Scholar
  60. Rescorla RA (1988) Behavioral studies of Pavlovian conditioning. Ann Rev Neurosci 11:329–352PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Rescorla RA, Freberg L (1978) The extinction of within-compound flavor associations. Learn Motiv 9:411–427CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Rescorla RA, Holland PC (1976) Some behavioral approaches to the study of learning. In: Bennett E, Rozensweig MR (eds) Neural mechanisms of learning and memory. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 165–192Google Scholar
  63. Rescorla RA, Holland PC (1982) Behavioral studies of associative learning in animals. Ann Rev Psychol 33:265–308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Rescorla RA, Solomon RL (1967) Two process learning theory: relationships between classical conditioning and instrumental learning. Psychol Rev 74:151–182PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Rolls BJ, Rolls ET (1982) Thirst. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  66. Rolls BJ, Wood RJ, Stevens RM (1978) Effects of palatability on body fluid homeostasis. Physiol Behav 20:15–19PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Rolls BJ, Rolls ET, Rowe EA (1981) Sensory specific satiety in man. Physiol Behav 27:137–142PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Seligman, MEP, Ives CE, Ames H, Mineka S (1970) Conditioned drinking and its failure to extinguish: avoidance, preparedness, or functional autonomy? J Comp Physiol Psychol 71:411–419PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Sheffield FD, Roby TB (1950) Reward value of a non-nutritive sweet taste. J Comp Physiol Psychol 43:471–481PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Shirley M (1929) Spontaneous activity. Psychol Bull 26:341–365CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Teitelbaum P (1966) The use of operant methods in the assessment and control of motivational states. In: Honig WK (ed) Operant behavior: areas of research and application. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  72. Tinbergen N (1951) The study of instinct. Clarendon Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  73. Tolman EC (1932) Purposive behavior in animals and men. Appleton-Century, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  74. Tolman EC, Brunswik E (1935) The organism and the causal texture of the environment. Psychol Rev 42:43–77CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. van Vort W, Smith GP (1987) Sham feeding experience produces a conditioned increase of meal size. Appetite 11:54–61Google Scholar
  76. Weisinger RS (1975) Conditioned and pseudoconditioned thirst and sodium appetite. In: Peters G, Fitzsimons JT, Peters-Haefeli L (ed) Control mechanisms of drinking. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, pp 148–154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Wikler A (1973) Conditioning of successive adaptive responses to the initial effects of drugs. Cond Reflex 8:193–210PubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Wolf AV (1958) Thirst: physiology of the urge to drink and problems of water lack. Thomas, Springfield ILGoogle Scholar
  79. Young PT (1955) The role of hedonic processes in motivation. In: Nebraska symposium on motivation. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln NEGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag London Limited 1991

Authors and Affiliations

  • P. C. Holland

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations