Skip to main content

British Animal Behaviour Studies in the Twentieth Century: Some Interdisciplinary Perspectives

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover The Psychology of the Human-Animal Bond

Abstract

An understanding of our relationship with animals, and of the uncertain boundary between our dependence on them and our exploitation of them, demands an awareness of the historical extent of the purposes of our interaction. An examination of this past relationship provides a context for a better assessment of the present-day importance many of us place on animals as other beings who ultimately have independent interests and a discreet power over our own human behaviour: they have become agents who affect the quality of our own lives. Our study, knowledge and manipulation of animal behaviour lie at the centre of the human–animal relationship, as demonstrated by the variety of situations in which attempts have been made to acquire a better understanding of animal behaviour in order to secure human interests. From the standpoint of the historian, this variety demands much interdisciplinary analysis concentrating on the late-nineteenth and the twentieth centuries. In this chapter, some British examples will be discussed in relation to developments in the United States, where scientific studies of animal behaviour soon stole the lead from Britain at the beginning of the last century.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 109.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 249.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    He was nevertheless “aware of the collectivist ideological uses of social insects”, and “employed ‘disinterested’ experimentation to cast doubts upon the utopian depictions of co-operative, altruistic communities of ants and bees” (Clark, 1997).

  2. 2.

    Lloyd Morgan’s desk became a forum for most of those involved in psychological research with animals in Britain until the 1930s. All types of investigators as well as some foreign workers corresponded with him. New publications were exchanged and admired, and points of disagreement discussed. The following correspondence is preserved in the Bristol University History Collection (as referenced). Charles Sherrington wrote in 1901 in appreciation of his newly received copy of Animal Behaviour (DM 612); and much later both he (in 1923) and, via his wife, an infirm Henry Head (in 1929) expressed great interest in Lloyd Morgan’s published studies of “emergent evolution” (DM 128/346 and DM 128/415). In 1913, Margaret Washburn referred to Lloyd Morgan’s criticisms of her The Animal Mind, to her misgivings about Watsonian behaviourism and to her appreciation of Lloyd Morgan’s Instinct and Experience (DM 128/290). Much further correspondence on each other’s work took place between Lloyd Morgan and C. S. Myers, E. B. Poulton (Hope Professor of Zoology at Oxford), William McDougall, J. A. Thomson and others (DM 128/various numbers and DM 612). Lloyd Morgan remained at the centre of a network of correspondence on matters concerning animal behaviour long after he ceased his own experiments.

  3. 3.

    Rollin (1989, pp. 67–68) observes: “One can indeed find elements of this reductionistic, ‘no frills’ philosophy throughout European culture. By the end of the nineteenth century, art, architecture, design, music, and literature had become extremely extravagant…. Much early twentieth-century culture can be seen as an attempt to eliminate or trim away that excess.”

  4. 4.

    See, for example, “Report by Professor Sir Ernest Rutherford FRS and Commander Cyprian Bridge RN, on Visit to the USA in company with French Scientific Mission, May 19th to July 9th, 1917.” BIR 28208/17. Public Record Office ADM 293/10.

  5. 5.

    The Admiralty’s use a little later of Cambridge University staff for hydrophone personnel selection and training, staff who were themselves responsible for overseeing animal work in the Cambridge Psychological Laboratory, demonstrates that a sufficient network existed for the employment of animal psychologists, had that been preferred. But the application of psychological expertise did not extend into this area, and in another, concerned with the identification, acceptance and treatment of what later came to be known as “shell shock”, the psychologists’ analysis was resisted.

  6. 6.

    L. C. Robbins (later Baron Robbins of Clare Market) was Chairman of the Committee on Higher Education (1961–1964), which was partly responsible for the major expansion and reforms of British university education in the 1960s.

  7. 7.

    Meanwhile, in the United States, attempts had been made during the Second World War to train pigeons to guide missiles: “The pigeon––an organism––is essentially an extremely reliable instrument, rugged in construction, simple and economical to obtain, and easily conditioned to be entirely predictable in behaviour [and which could] be made into a machine, from all practical points of view” (B.F. Skinner cited by Capshew, 1993, pp. 850–851).

  8. 8.

    This involved the issue of guidelines for the use of animals in research to members and correspondents of the ASAB (1981 and 1986), BPS (1985) and EPS (1986).

  9. 9.

    For example, following pressure from its membership, a leading ethologist, Patrick Bateson, was commissioned by the British landowning conservation charity, the National Trust, to assess the suffering occasioned by hunting stags with hounds on its land, so that Trust policy could be informed and decided upon (Bateson, 1997).

References

  • Allen, E. J. (1917). Report upon experiments on the hearing powers of sea-lions under water, and on the possibility of training these animals as submarine trackers. B.I.R. 30051/17. London: Admiralty Board of Invention and Research. Public Record Office ADM 293/5, 450–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, P. P. G. (1997). The behavioural and physiological effects of culling red deer. London: Council of the National Trust.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boakes, R. A. (1984). From Darwin to behaviourism: Psychology and the minds of animals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadhurst, P. L. (1963). The science of animal behaviour. Harmondsworth: Pelican Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Capshew, J. H. (1993). Engineering behaviour: Project Pigeon, World War II, and the conditioning of B.F. Skinner. Technology and Culture, 34, 835–857.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, J. F. M. (1997). ‘The ants were duly visited’: Making sense of John Lubbock, scientific naturalism and the senses of social insects. British Journal for the History of Science, 30, 151–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danziger, K. (1987). Social context and investigative practice in early twentieth-century psychology. In M. G. Ash & W. R. Woodward (Eds.), Psychology in twentieth-century thought and society. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. R. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London: John Murray.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dewsbury, D. A. (1984). Comparative psychology in the twentieth century. Stroudsburg: Hutchinson Ross.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, M. W. (1981). Experimental psychology, animal rights, welfare and ethics. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 17, 80–84.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb, G. (1979). Comparative psychology and ethology. In E. Hearst (Ed.), The first century of experimental psychology. Hillsdale, IN: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, P. H. (1963). The Morgan–Romanes controversy: A contradiction in the history of comparative psychology. Proceedings of the Montana Academy of Sciences, 23, 225–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, P. H. (1967). Spalding and his influence on research in developmental behaviour. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 3, 168–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, P. H. (1968). Prerequisite to an analysis of behaviorism: The conscious automaton theory from Spalding to William James. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 4, 365–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gwynne Jones, H. (1969). Clinical psychology. Supplement to the Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 22, 21–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour, I, II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hearnshaw, L. S. (1964). A short history of British psychology 1840–1940. London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hearnshaw, L. S. (1966, 5 May). The comparative psychology of mental development (L. T. Hobhouse Memorial Trust Lecture no. 36). London: Bedford College, University of London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobhouse, L. T. (1902). Articles under the title “The diversions of a psychologist”. The Pilot: a weekly review of politics, literature, and learning, 5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobhouse, L. T. (1915). Mind in evolution (2nd Ed.). London: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hume, C. W. (1959). In praise of anthropomorphism. The UFAW Courier, 1, 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, H. M. (1979). Animal learning and behavior theory. In E. Hearst (Ed.), The first century of experimental psychology. Hillsdale, IN: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd Morgan, C. (1894). Introduction to comparative psychology. London: Scott.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd Morgan, C. (1896). Habit and instinct. London: Arnold.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd Morgan, C. (1900). Animal behaviour. London: Arnold.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd Morgan, C. (1923). Emergent evolution. London: Williams and Norgate.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd Morgan, C. (1930). The animal mind. London: Arnold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovelock, J. (1979). Gaia: A new look at life on Earth. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lubbock, J. A. (1882). Ants, bees, and wasps. New York: Appleton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackenzie, B. D. (1977). Behaviourism and the limits of scientific method. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Donnell, J. M. (1985). The origins of behaviorism: American psychology, 1870–1920. New York: New York University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, D. and Ryder, R. D. (Eds.) (1979). Animals’ rights – a symposium. Fontwell: Centaur.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robbins, L. C. (1963). (chairman) Report of the Committee on Higher Education. Cmnd 2154. London: HMSO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rollin, B. E. (1989). The unheeded cry: Animal consciousness, animal pain and science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romanes, G. J. (1878). Evening discourse delivered before the British Association, Dublin. London: Taylor and Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romanes, G. J. (1882). Animal intelligence. London: Kegan Paul, Trench.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romanes, G. J. (1883). Mental evolution in animals. London: Kegan Paul, Trench.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romanes, G. J. (1885). Jelly-fish, star-fish and sea-urchins. Being a research on primitive nervous systems. London: Kegan Paul, Trench.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Romanes, E. G. (1896). The life and letters of George John Romanes. London: Longmans, Green.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ryder, R. D. (2000). Animal revolution: Changing attitudes towards speciesism. Oxford: Berg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, B. (1981). History of animal behaviour. In D. MacFarland (Ed.), Oxford companion to animal behaviour. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spalding, D. A. (1872). On instinct. Nature, 6(154), 485–486.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spalding, D. A. (1873). Instinct; with original observations on young animals. Macmillan’s Magazine, 27, 282–293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorpe, W. H. (1927-1984). William Homan Thorpe: Papers. Add. MS 8784/M13. Cambridge: Cambridge University Library Department of Manuscripts and University Archives.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorpe, W. H. (1956). Some implications of the study of animal behaviour. The Advancement of Science, 13, 42–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N. (1940). Correspondence with D. Lack, 26 February 1940. In the David Lack Papers, Edward Grey Institute, Oxford, item 155. Cited by Durant, J.R. in The making of ethology: the Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour, 1936 to 1986. Animal Behaviour 1986, 34, 1601–1616.

    Google Scholar 

  • United Kingdom Parliament. (1921). Parliamentary Debates (Commons), 144, column 1243. London: HMSO.

    Google Scholar 

  • United Kingdom Parliament. (1921 and 1922). Report from the select committee on performing animals, together with the proceedings of the committee and minutes of evidence. London: HMSO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valentine, E. R. (2006). Beatrice Edgell: Pioneer woman psychologist. New York: Nova.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warden, C. J. (1928). The development of modern comparative psychology. Quarterly Review of Biology, 3, 486–522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiskrantz, L. (1985). Categorization, cleverness and consciousness. Philosophical Transactions of The Royal Society, B, 308, 3–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. A. H. (1999). Encouragements and constraints in the development of experimental animal behaviour studies in Great Britain since the late nineteenth century. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, University of Leicester, Leicester, England.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. A. H. (2001a). A ‘precipitous dégringolade’? The uncertain progress of British comparative psychology in the twentieth century. In G. C. Bunn, A. D. Lovie, & G. D. Richards (Eds.), Psychology in Britain: Historical essays and personal reflections. Leicester: British Psychological Society, in association with the Science Museum, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. A. H. (2001b). Sea lions, greasepaint and the U-boat threat: Admiralty scientists turn to the music hall in 1916. Notes and Records of The Royal Society, 55, 425–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. A. H. (2002a). Animal psychology and ethology in Britain and the emergence of professional concern for the concept of ethical cost. Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 33, 235–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. A. H. (2002b). Experimental animal behaviour studies: The loss of initiative in Britain 100 years ago. History of Science, 40, 291–320.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. A. H. (2003). British female academics and comparative psychology: Attempts to establish a research niche in the early twentieth century. History of Psychology, 6, 89–109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. A. H. (2004). The public relations of experimental animal psychology in Britain in the 1970s. Contemporary British History, 18, 27–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. A. H. (2006). Avian anti-submarine warfare proposals in Britain, 1915-18: The Admiralty and Thomas Mills. International Journal of Naval History, 5, 1–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. A. H. (2008). Politics, press and the performing animals controversy in early twentieth-century Britain. Anthrozoös, 21, 317–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. A. H. (2009a). ‘Crank legislators’, ‘faddists’ and professionals’ defence of animal performance in 1920s Britain. Early Popular Visual Culture, 7, 83–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. A. H. (2009b). Racial prejudice and the performing animals controversy in early twentieth-century Britain. Society and Animals. Journal of Human-Animal Studies, 17, 149–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, S. (1988). Monkeys, men and missiles. An autobiography 1946-1988. London: Collins.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David A. H. Wilson .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Wilson, D.A.H. (2011). British Animal Behaviour Studies in the Twentieth Century: Some Interdisciplinary Perspectives. In: Blazina, C., Boyraz, G., Shen-Miller, D. (eds) The Psychology of the Human-Animal Bond. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-9761-6_2

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics