Skip to main content

Socioemotional Systems and Neo-Eriksonian Dangers

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Development and Causality
  • 1789 Accesses

Abstract

To this point, several major socioaffective acquisitions in the course of development have been discussed, especially those of the 25 steps in Neo-Eriksonian development across the lifespan and a model of the basic emotions early in life. There is no contradiction here, because just as cognitive development has global and more ­specific acquisitions, so does the realm of socioemotional development.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Bialystok, E. (1992). Symbolic representation of letters and numbers. Cognitive Development, 7, 301–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blake, J., & Dolgoy, S. J. (1993). Gestural development and its relation to cognition during the transition to language. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 17, 87–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bullock, M., & Lutkenhaus, P. (1988). The development of volitional behavior in the toddler years. Child Development, 59, 664–674.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009a). Anger is an approach-related affect: Evidence and implications. Psychological Bulletin, 13, 183–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009b). Anger and approach: Reply to Watson (2009) and to Tomarken and Zald (2009). Psychological Bulletin, 135, 215–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohn, J. F., & Tronick, E. Z. (1983). Three-month-old infants’ reaction to simulated maternal depression. Child Development, 54, 185–193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohn, J. F., & Tronick, E. Z. (1988). Mother-infant face-to-face interaction: Influence is bidirectional and unrelated to periodic cycles in either partner’s behavior. Developmental Psychology, 24, 386–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Demetriou, A., Efklides, A., & Platsidou, M. (1993). The architecture and dynamics of developing mind: Experiential structuralism as a frame for unifying cognitive developmental theories. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58 (5/6, Serial No. 234).

    Google Scholar 

  • Fogel, A. (1991). Infancy: Infant, family, and society (2nd ed.). Saint Paul: West.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M. A. K., & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howe, M. L., & Courage, M. L. (1993). On resolving the enigma of infantile amnesia. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 305–326.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Legerstee, M. (1990). Infants use multimodel information to imitate speech sounds. Infant Behavior and Development, 3, 343–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, J. M. (1992). How to build a baby: II. Conceptual primitives. Psychological Review, 99, 587–604.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mascolo, M. F., & Fischer, K. W. (2010). The dynamic development of thinking, feeling and ­acting over the lifespan. In R. M. Lerner & W. F. Overton (Eds.), Handbook of lifespan ­development: Vol. 1. Cognitive, biology and methods. Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1989). Imitation in newborn infants: Exploring the range of gestures imitated and the underlying mechanisms. Developmental Psychology, 25, 954–962.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1992). Early imitation within a functional framework: The importance of person identity, movement, and development. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 479–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1994). Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 83–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Messinger, D., & Fogel, A. (1990). The role of referential gazing in object exchange. Paper ­presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies, Montreal, April.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J. (1990). Emotions, development and psychotherapy: A dialectical constructivist perspective. In J. D. Safran & L. S. Greenberg (Eds.), Emotion, psychotherapy and change (pp. 302–335). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P. (1989). Object manipulation and exploration in 2- to 5-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 25, 871–884.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P., & Senders, S. J. (1991). Active touch in infancy: Action systems in development. In M. J. Weiss & P. R. Zelazo (Eds.), Newborn attention: Biological constraints and the influence of experience (pp. 412–422). Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanley, D. J., & Meyer, J. P. (2009). Two-dimensional affective space: A new approach to orienting the axes. Emotion, 9, 214–237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., & Fogel, A. (1989). Toward an action-based theory of infant development. In J. Lockman & N. Hazen (Eds.), Action in social context: Perspectives on early development (pp. 23–64). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. S. (1973). Smiling, cooing, and “the game”. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 18, 323–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolff, P. H. (1987). The development of behavioral states and the expression of emotions in early infancy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, G. (1990a). Early neuropsychological development: Lateralization of functions – ­hemispheric specialization. In C. A. Hauert (Ed.), Developmental psychology: Cognitive, ­perceptuo-motor and neuropsychological perspectives (pp. 113–181). Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Young, G. (1990b). The development of hemispheric and manual specialization. In G. E. Hammond (Ed.), Cerebral control of speech and limb movements (pp. 79–139). Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Young, G. (1997). Adult development, therapy, and culture: A postmodern synthesis. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gerald Young .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Young, G. (2011). Socioemotional Systems and Neo-Eriksonian Dangers. In: Development and Causality. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-9422-6_20

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics