Skip to main content

Toward an Integrated Science of Morality

Linking Mind, Society and Culture

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover Handbook of the Sociology of Morality

Abstract

This chapter constructs a sociological mantle for morality and the mind by specifying broad theoretical traditions in the sociological literature on structure, culture, and interaction amenable to neuropsychological models of morality. Moral action does not take place in a vacuum; people are embedded in culture and social structure. We belong to families, groups, organizations, societies that shape and are shaped by our actions, thoughts and feelings. Research in sociology is well established to provide the essential links between human mind – extensively focused on within moral psychology – with society and culture. A comprehensive science of morality not only tests the neurological mechanisms involved in morality in artificial settings but is anchored in sociological understandings about how morality is shaped and activated in everyday human life.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 169.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The anterior cingulate cortex is involved in several important affective and cognitive tasks including but not limited to autonomic activity, conditioned emotional learning, internal emotional responses, motivation, motor activity and goal directed behavior (Bush et al. 2000, Devinsky et al. 1995), selective attention (Casebeer and Churchland 2003), and social cooperative action (Rilling et al. 2001).

  2. 2.

    Warfield-Rawls (1987) suggests that it is the unexpected in basic interaction that makes up the moral (see her chapter, this volume).

  3. 3.

    We do not advocate for an approach that perilously constructs an over-socialized perspective of persons. Wrong’s (1961) point is quite clear, and just, and socialization is but a reduced-form construct explaining how social information is acquired and retained.

  4. 4.

    In Alexander and Smith’s (1993) terms, binaries are dualistically organized: good versus bad, right versus wrong, clean versus dirty. We suggest that the world is a far more complex place. This approach does not reflect gradations of evaluation.

  5. 5.

    Interested parties might look to New Institutionalism in organizational sociology, which has long investigated culture as shaping and directing lines of action (see Powell and DiMaggio 1991, Friedland and Alford 1991 for theoretic orientation; Thornton and William 1999, Haveman and Rao 1997, Rao et al. 2003). More recently, new institutional scholarship has loosened the constraints of theory, acknowledging discursive processes between culture and interpretation by organizational actors. Persons filter, construct and strategically utilize this cultural information to direct action and pursue interests (see Hallett 2003, Hallett and Ventresca 2006a, b, Binder 2007, Hallett 2010).

  6. 6.

    Only suggestively, and guided by Turner and Stets (2006): dramaturgical theories are rooted in capturing how culture acts to define which emotions should be both experienced and expressed in a given situation (e.g., Goffman 1959, 1967, Hochschild 1983, Scheff 1988). Symbolic interactionist theories are rooted in understanding the relation of the self and identity to emotions through interaction. The self and identity are embedded in, constructed and reproduced through interaction (e.g., Mead 1934, Burke 1991, McCall and Simmons 1978, Stryker 1980, Turner 2002). Power and status theories generally connote the effects of status processes and power dynamics in the evocation of emotions or emotional states (Kemper and Collins 1990, Bianchi 2004, Ridgeway and Johnson 1990, Ridgeway 1994). Finally, exchange theories focus on exchange processes between actors (necessary acts in social life), in which exchange can be conceptualized in a bevy of manners. Importantly, each of these theories of exchange examines how exchange processes evoke or construct emotions and feeling states (e.g. Lawler 2001, Molm 1997, Cook and Emerson 1978, Kollack 1994, Lawler and Yoon 1998).

References

  • Adolphs, R., F. Gosselin, T. W. Buchanan, D. Tranel, P. Schyns, and A. R. Damasio. 2005. “A mechanism for impaired fear recognition after amygdala damage.” Nature 433:68–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adolphs, R., D. Tranel, and A. R. Damasio. 1998. “The Human Amygdala in Social Judgment”. Nature 393:470–474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adolphs, R. 2009. “The Social Brain: Neural Basis of Social Knowledge”. Annual Review of Psychology 60: 693–716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, J., eds. 1988. Durkheimian Sociology: Cultural Studies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, J., and P. Smith. 1993. “The Discourse of American Civil Society: A New Proposal for Cultural Studies”. Theory and Society 22(2):151–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beauregard, M., J. Lévesque, and P. Bourgouin. 2001. “Neural correlates of conscious self-regulation of emotion”. Journal of Neuroscience 21(18):1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, P., and T. Luckmann. 1966. The Social Construction of Reality. New York: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergmann, J. R. 1998. “Introduction: Morality in Discourse”. Research on Language and Social Interaction 31(3&4):279–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bianchi, A. 2004. “Rejecting Others’ Influence: Negative Sentiment and Status in Task Groups”. Sociological Perspectives 47:339–355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biggart, N. W., and T. D. Beamish. 2003. “The Economic Sociology of Conventions: Habit, Custom, Practice, and Routine in Market Order”. Annual Review of Sociology 29: 443–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binder, A. 2007. “For Love and Money: Organizations’ Creative Responses to Multiple Environmental Logics”. Theoretical Sociology 36: 547–571.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boltanski, L., and L. Thevenot. 2006. On Justification: Economies of Worth, translated by Catherine Porter. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boorman, S. A., and H. White. 1976. “Social Structure from Multiple Networks: II. Role Structures”. American Journal of Sociology 81: 1384–1446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P. 1984. Distinction: A Social Critique of the Judgment of Taste, translated by R. Nice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P. 1990 (1980). “Structure, Habitus, Practices”. PP. 52–65 in The Logic of Practice. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burke, P. 1991. “Identity Processes and Social Stress”. American Sociological Review 56: 836–849.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bush G., P. Luu, and M. I. Posner. 2000. “Cognitive and Emotional Influences in Anterior Cingulate Cortex”. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4(6): 215–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carley, K. 1991. “A Theory of Group Stability”. American Sociological Review 56: 331–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casebeer, W. D., and P. S. Churchland. 2003. “The Neural Mechanisms of Moral Cognition: A Multiple-Aspect Approach to Moral Judgment and Decision-Making”. Biology and Philosophy 18: 169–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, J. 1988. “Social Capital in the Creation of Human Capital”. The American Journal of Sociology 94: S95–S120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, R. 1981. “On the Microfoundations of Macrosociology”. American Journal of Sociology 86: 984–1014.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, K. 1977. “Exchange and Power in Networks of Interorganizational Relations”. The Sociological Quarterly 18: 62–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, K., and R. Emerson. 1978. “Power, Equity and Commitment in Exchange Networks”. American Sociological Review 43: 721–739.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, Karen, Richard Emerson, Mary Gillmore, and Toshio Yamagishi. 1983. “The Distribution of Power in Exchange Networks: Theory and Experimental Results”. American Journal of Sociology 89(2): 275–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, K., and J. Whitmeyer. 1992. “Two Approaches to Social Structure: Exchange Theory and Network Analysis”. Annual Review of Sociology 18: 109–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dalgleish, T. 2004. “The Emotional Brain”. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 5: 582–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. 1994. Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: Avon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R., D. Tranel, and H. Damasio. 1990. “Individuals with sociopathic behavior caused by frontal damage fail to respond autonomically to social stimuli.” Behavioural Brain Research 41: 81–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R., D. Tranel, and H. Damasio. 1991. “Somatic markers and the guidance of behaviour: theory and preliminary testing.” PP. 217–229 in Frontal Lobe Function and Dysfunction, edited by H. S. Levin, H. M. Eisenberg and A. L. Benton. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R., K. M. Putnam, and C. L. Larson. 2000. “Dysfunction in the Neural Circuitry of Emotion Regulation: A Possible Prelude to Violence”. Science 289: 591–594.

    Google Scholar 

  • DiMaggio, P. 1997. “Culture and Cognition”. Annual Review of Sociology 23: 263–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiMaggio, P. J., and W. Powell. 1983. “The Iron Cage Revisited: Institutional Isomorphism and Collective Rationality in Organizational Fields.” American Sociological Review 48: 147–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • DiMaggio, P. J., and W. Powell. 1991. “Introduction to the new institutionalism.” PP. 1–38 in The New Institutionalism in Organizational Analysis, edited by W. Powell and P. J. DiMaggio. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Douglas, M. 1986. How Institutions Think. Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drew, P. 1998. “Complaints About Transgressions and Misconduct”. Research on Language and Social Interaction 31(3&4): 295–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emerson, R. 1962. “Power-Dependence Relations”. American Sociological Review 27(1): 31–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emirbayer, M. 1997. “Manifesto for a Relational Sociology”. American Journal of Sociology 103(2): 281–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emirbayer, M., and J. Goodwin. 1994. “Network Analysis, Culture, and the Problem of Agency”. American Journal of Sociology 99(6):1411–1454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. 1997. “Worldviews or Social Groups as the Source of Moral Value Attitudes: Implications for the Culture Wars Thesis”. Sociological Forum 12(3):371–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fligstein, N. 1987. “The Intraorganizational Power Struggle: Rise of Finance Personnel to Top Leadership in Large Corporations, 1919–1979.” American Sociological Review 52: 44–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fligstein, N. 1997. “Social Skill and Institutional Theory”. The American Behavioral Scientist 40: 397–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fligstein, N. 2001. “Social Skill and the Theory of Fields”. Sociological Theory 19(2): 105–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedland, R., and R. Alford. 1991. “Bringing Society Back In: Symbols, Practices, and Institutional Contradictions”. PP. 232–263 in The New Institutionalism in Organizational Analysis, edited by W. Powell, and P. DiMaggio. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. 1959. The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. 1967. Interaction Ritual: Essays on Face-to-Face Behavior. Garden City, NY: Anchor Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D. 2007a. “The Secret Joke of Kant’s Soul”. PP. 59–66 in Moral Psychology, Vol. 3, edited by W. Sinnott-Armstrong. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D. 2007b. “Why are VMPFC Patients More Utilitarian? A Dual-Process Theory of Moral Judgment Explains”. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11(8):322–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D., R. B. Sommerville, L. E. Nystrom, J. M. Darley, and J. D. Cohen. 2001. “An fMRI Investigation of Emotional Engagement in Moral Judgment.” Science 293: 2105–2108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D., and J. Haidt. 2002. “How (and Where) Does Moral Judgment Work?” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6(12): 517–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D., L. E. Nystrom, A. D. Engell, J. M. Darley, and J. D. Cohen. 2004. “The Neural Bases of Cognitive Conflict and Control in Moral Judgment”. Neuron 44(2):389–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D., S. Morelli, K. Lowenberg, L. Nystrom, and J. Cohen. 2008. “Cognitive Load Selectively Interferes with Utilitarian Moral Judgment”. Cognition 107(3): 1144–1154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. 2001. “The Emotional Dog and Its Rational Tail: A Social Intuitionist Approach to Moral Judgment.” Psychological Review 108: 814–834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. 2008. “Morality”. Perspectives on Psychological Science 3(1): 65–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J., S. H. Koller, and M. G. Dias. 1993. “Affect, Culture, and Morality, or Is It Wrong to Eat Your Dog?” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65(4): 613–628.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. et al. 2007. “The New Synthesis in Moral Psychology”. Science 316: 998–1002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hallett, T. 2003. “Symbolic Power and Organizational Culture.” Sociological Theory 21: 128–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hallett, T., and M. Ventresca. 2006a. “Inhabited Institutions: Social Interactions and Organizational Forms in Gouldner’s Patterns of Industrial Bureaucracy”. Theoretical Sociology 35: 213–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hallett, T., and M. Ventresca. 2006b. “How Institutions Form: Loose Coupling as Mechanism in Gouldner’s Patterns of Industrial Bureaucracy”. American Behavioral Scientist 49(7): 908–924.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hallett, T. 2010 (in press). “The Myth Incarnate: Institutional Recoupling and Turmoil in an Urban Elementary School”. Obtained with Permission from Author via email Correspondence in August 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D. 2006. Moral Minds: The Nature of Right and Wrong. New York: HarperCollins Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D. 2007. “What’s fair? The Unconscious Calculus of Our Moral Faculty”. Empathy and Fairness 278: 41–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D. 2009. “The Possibility of Impossible Cultures”. Nature 460: 190–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D., and L. Young. 2008. Modules, Minds and Morality. In Hormones and Social Behavior. Verlag, Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haveman, H., and H. Rao. 1997. “Structuring a Theory of Moral Sentiments: Institutional and Organizational Coevolution in the Early Thrift Industry”. American Journal of Sociology 102: 1606–1651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hitlin, S. 2008. Moral Selves, Evil Selves: The Social Psychology of Conscience. New York: Palgrave MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hochschild, A. 1983. The Managed Heart: The Commercialization of Human Feeling. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofstede, G. 1991. Cultures and Organizations: Software of the Mind. London: McGraw-Hill Book Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, D. 1997. “Selves and Cultured: As Told By An Anthropologist Who Lacks a Soul”. PP. 160–190 in Self and Identity: Fundamental Issues, edited by R. Ashmore, and L. Jussim. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howard, J., and D. Renfrow. 2003. “Social Cognition”. PP. 259–281 in Handbook of Social Psychology, edited by J. Delamater. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huebner, B., S. Dwyer, and M. D. Hauser. 2008. “The Role of Emotion in Moral Psychology”. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13(1): 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunter, J. 1991. Culture Wars: The Struggle to Define America. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Izard, C. 2007. “Basic Emotions, Natural Kinds, Emotion Schemas, and a New Paradigm”. Personality and Psychological Science 2: 260–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Izard, C. 2009. “Emotion Theory and Research: Highlights, Unanswered Questions, and Emerging Issues”. Annual Review of Psychology 60: 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackall, R. 1988. Moral Mazes: The World of Corporate Managers. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jepperson, R. 1991. “Institutions, Institutional Effects, and Institutionalism”. PP. 143–163 in The New Institutionalism in Organizational Analysis, edited by W. Powell, and P. DiMaggio. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, J. 2004. “Endogenous Explanations in the Sociology of Culture”. Annual Review of Sociology 30: 335–357.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kemper, T., ed. 1990. Research Agendas in the Sociology of Emotions. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kemper, T., and R. Collins. 1990. “Dimensions of Microinteraction”. American Sociological Review 96: 32–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. 1969. “Stage and Sequence: The Cognitive-Developmental Approach to Socialization”. In Handbook of Socialization Theory and Research, edited by D. A. Goslin. Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. 1971. “From is to Ought: How to Commit the Naturalistic Fallacy and Get Away with It in the Study of Moral Development”. PP. 151–235 in Cognitive Developmental Epistemology, edited by T. Mischel. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kollack, P. 1994. “The Emergence of Exchange Structures: An Experimental Study of Uncertainty, Commitment, and Trust”. The American Journal of Sociology 100(2): 313–345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kondo, D. 1990. Crafting Selves: Power, Gender, and Discourses of Identity in a Japanese Workplace. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamont, M. 1992. Money, Morals, and Manners: The Culture of the French and the American Upper-Middle Class. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamont, M. 2000. The Dignity of Working Men: Morality and the Boundaries of Race, Class and Immigration. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamont, M. 2001. “Culture and Identity”. PP. 171–185 in Handbook of Sociological Theory, edited by J. Turner. New York: Kluwer Academics/Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamont, M., and M. Fournier, eds. 1992. Cultivating Differences: Symbolic Boundaries and the Making of Inequality. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamont, M., and V. Molnar. 2002. “The Study of Boundaries in the Social Sciences”. Annual Review of Sociology 28: 167–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lane, R. D., E. M. Reiman, M. M. Bradley, P. J. Lang, G. L. Ahern, R. J. Davidsona, and G. E. Schwartz. 1997. “Neuroanatomical Correlates Of Pleasant and Unpleasant Emotion.” Neuropsychologia 35(11): 1437–1444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawler, E. 2001. “An Affect Theory of Social Exchange”. American Journal of Sociology 107: 321–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawler, E., and J. Yoon. 1998. “Network Structure and Emotion in Exchange Relations”. American Sociological Review 63: 871–894.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J E. 2000. “Emotion Circuits in the Brain”. Annual Review of Neuroscience 23: 155–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. E. 2002. Synaptic Self-How Our Brains Become Who We Are. New York: Viking.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. E. 2003. “The Self: Clues from the Brain”. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1001: 295–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacLean, P. D. 1990. The Triune Brain in Evolution: Role in Paleocerebral Functions. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J. 1992. Cultures in Organizations: Three Perspectives. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Massey, D. 2002. “A Brief History of Human Society: The Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life: 2001 Presidential Address”. American Sociological Review 67(1): 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maynard, D. 1998. “Praising Versus Blaming the Messenger: Moral Issues in Deliveries of Good and Bad News”. Research on Language and Social Interaction 31(3&4): 359–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McAdam, D. 1995. Doug McAdam, “`Initiator’ and `Spinoff’ Movements: Diffusion Processes in Protest Cycles.” In Repertoires and Cycles of Collective Action, edited by M. Traugott. Durham, North Carolina: Duke University.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCall, G., and J. L. Simmons. 1978. Identities and Interactions. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McEwen, B. S. 2007. “Physiology and Neurobiology of Stress and Adaptation: Central Role of the Brain”. Physiological Review 87: 873–904.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mead, G. H. 1934. Mind, Self and Society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Melucci, A. 1996. Challenging Codes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, J., and B. Rowan. 1977. “Institutionalized Organizations: Formal Structure as Myth and Ceremony”. American Journal of Sociology 83: 333–363.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., R. de Oliviera-Souza, P. J. Eslinger, I. E. Bramati, J. Mourao-Miranda, P. Angelo Andreiuolo, and L. Pessoa. 2002. “The Neural Correlates of Moral Sensitivity: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Investigation of Basic and Moral Emotions”. The Journal of Neuroscience 22(7): 2730–2736.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., R. de Oliviera-Souza, and P. J. Eslinger. 2003. “Morals and the Human Brain: A Working Model”. Neuroreport 14: 299–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., R. Zahn, R. de Oliviera-Souza, F. Krueger, and J. Grafman. 2005. “The Neural Basis of Human Moral Cognition”. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6: 799–809.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., R. de Oliviera-Souza, G. J. Garrido, I. E. Bramati, E. M. A. Caparelli-Daquer, M. L. M. F. Pavia, R. Zahn, and J. Grafman. 2007. “The Self as a Moral Agent: Linking the Neural Bases of Social Agency and Moral Sensitivity”. Social Neuroscience 2(3–4): 336–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molm, L. 1997. Coercive Power in Exchange. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, R. 1976. “The Production of Culture: A Prolegomenon”. PP. 7–22 in The Production of Culture, edited by R. A. Peterson. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, R. 1979. “Revitalizing the Culture Concept”. Annual Review of Sociology 5: 137–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, R., and N. Anand. 2004. “The Production of Culture Perspective”. Annual Review of Sociology 30: 311–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phelps, E. A., and J. E. LeDoux. 2005. “Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: from animal models to human behavior.” Neuron 48:175–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. 1965. The Moral Judgment of the Child. New York: Free Press. (Original work published in 1932).

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell, W. 1990. “Neither Market Nor Hierarchy: Network Forms of Organization”. PP. 295–336 in Research in Organizational Behavior, vol. 12, edited by B. Staw and L. L. Cummings. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell, W., and P. J. DiMaggio. 1991. The New Institutionalism In Organizational Analysis. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rao, H., P. Monin, and R. Durand. 2003. “Institutional Change in ToqueVille: Nouvelle Cuisine as an Identity Movement in French Gastronomy”. American Journal of Sociology 108: 795–843.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rao, H., C. Morrill, and M. N. Zald. 2000. “Power Plays: How Social Movements and Collective Action Create New Organizational Forms”. Research in Organizational Behavior 22: 237–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rawls, A. W. 1987. “The Interaction Order Sui Generis: Goffman’s Contribution to Social Theory”. Sociological Theory 5(2): 136–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ridgeway, C. 1994. “Affect”. PP. 205–230 in Group Processes: Sociological Analyses, edited by M. Foschi, and E. Lawler. Chicago: Nelson-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ridgeway, C., and C. Johnson. 1990. “What is the Relationship Between Socioemotional Behavior and Status in Task Groups?” American Journal of Sociology 95: 1189–1212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rilling, J. K., D. A. Gutmana, T. R. Zeha, G. Pagnonia, G. S. Bernsa, and D. Clinton. 2001. “A Neural Basis for Social Cooperation”. Neuron 35: 395–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rusak B., and I. Zucker. 1979. “Neural Regulation of Circadian-Rhythms”. Physiological Reviews 59(3): 449–526.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheff, T. 1988. “Shame and Conformity: The Deference-Emotion System”. American Sociological Review 53: 395–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M. W., S. C. Woods, D. Porte, Jr, R. J. Seeley, and D. G. Baskin. 2000. “Central Nervous System Control of Food Intake”. Nature 404: 661–671.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R. 1994. ““You’re Not Sick, You’re Just in Love”: An Attributional Theory of Motivation and Emotion”. PP. 32–44 in The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions, edited by P. Ekman, and R. Davidson. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sewell, W. 1992. “A Theory of Structure: Duality, Agency, and Transformation”. The American Journal of Sociology 98: 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simpson Jr. J. R., W. C. Drevets, A. Z. Snyder, D. A. Gusnard, and M. E. Raichle. 2001. “Emotion-induced Changes in Human Medial Prefrontal Cortex: II. During Anticipatory Anxiety”. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 98(2): 688–693.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. 2003. Moral, Believing Animals: Human Personhood and Culture. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Smith-Lovin, L. 1995. “The Sociology of Affect and Emotion”. PP. 118–148 in Sociological Perspectives on Social Psychology, edited by K. Cook, G. A. Fine, and J. House. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L. 1992. “Memory and the Hippocampus – A Synthesis from Findings with Rats, Monkeys, and Humans”. Psychological Review 99(2): 195–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L., and S. Zolamorgan. 1991. “The Medial Temporal-Lobe Memory System”. Science 253(5026): 1380–1386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stryker, S. 1980. Symbolic Interactionism: A Social Structural Version. Caldwell, NJ: The Blackburn Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swidler, A. 1986. “Culture in Action: Symbols and Strategies”. American Sociological Review 51: 273–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tajfel, H., and J. Turner. 1985. “The Social Identity Theory of Intergroup Behavior”. PP. 7–24 in Psychology of Intergroup Relations, edited by S. Worchel, and W. Austin. Chicago: Nelson-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thornton, P. H., and O. William. 1999. “Institutional Logics and the Historical Contingency of Power in Organizations: Executive Succession in the Higher Education Publishing Industry, 1958–1990.” American Journal of Sociology 105:801–843.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tilly, C. 1992. “How to Detect, Describe, and Explain Repertoires of Contention”. Working Paper No. 150. Center for the Study of Social Change. New School For Social Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, J. 2000. On the Origins of Human Emotions: A Sociological Inquiry into the Evolution of Human Affects. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, J. 2002. Face-to-Face: Toward a Sociological Theory of Interpersonal Behavior. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, J., and J. Stets. 2005. The Sociology of Emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, J., and J. Stets. 2006. “Sociological Theories of Human Emotions”. Annual Review of Sociology 32: 25–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, J., and J. Stets. 2007. “Moral Emotions”. PP. 544–566 in Handbook of the Sociology of Emotions, edited by J. Turner, and J. Stets. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vaisey, S. 2008. “Socrates, Skinner, and Aristotle: Three Ways of Thinking About Culture in Action”. Sociological Forum 23(3): 603–613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, B. 1983. “Network Analysis: Some Basic Principles”. PP. 155–200 in Sociological Theory 1983, edited by R. Collins. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wexler, B. E. 2006. Brain and Culture: Neurobiology, Ideology, Social Change. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wrong, D. H. 1961. “The Oversocialized Conception of Man in Modern Sociology.” American Sociological Review 26: 183–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wuthnow, R. 1996. “The Restructuring of American Religion: Further Evidence.” Sociological Inquiry 66: 303–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rengin Firat .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Firat, R., McPherson, C.M. (2010). Toward an Integrated Science of Morality. In: Hitlin, S., Vaisey, S. (eds) Handbook of the Sociology of Morality. Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-6896-8_19

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics