Advertisement

Neurosociology pp 129-156 | Cite as

The Self in Neuroscience and Social Psychology

  • David D. Franks
Chapter

Abstract

Less than 40 years ago the reigning opinion in much of social psychology was that thinking about the human self was a waste of time. Fifteen to twenty years ago this was also a popular view among neuroscientists. The study of self is now a thriving enterprise in both fields. In 1999, Antonio Damasio published The Feeling of What Happens which put the self on center stage in his thinking.

Keywords

Brain Area Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Soccer Player Medial Temporal Lobe Medial Prefrontal Cortex 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Allman, J., A. Hakeem and K. Watson (2002). Two phylogenetic specializations in the human brain. The Neuroscientist, 8(4), 335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84(2), 191–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baumeister, R. (1986). Identity: Cultural change and the struggle for self. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Bruner, J. S. (1961). The act of discovery. Harvard Educational Review, 31(1), 21–32.Google Scholar
  5. Burton, R. A. (2008). On being certain: Believing you are right even when you’re not. New York: St. Martin’s Press.Google Scholar
  6. Cooley, C. H. (1902). The looking-glass self. In Human nature and the social order. New York: Scribner.Google Scholar
  7. Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error and the future of human life. Scientific American, 271(4), 144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Damasio, A. R. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
  9. Damasio, A. (2003). Feelings of emotion and the self. In J. LeDoux, J. Debiec, H. Moss (Eds.), The self from soul to brain, Vol. 1001. New York: Annals of The New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
  10. Decety, J. and T. Chaminade (2003). When the self represents the other: A new cognitive neuroscience view on psychological identification. Consciousness and Cognition, 12(4), 577.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dennett, D. C. (2003). Freedom evolves. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
  12. Dewey, J. (2004). Reconstruction in philosophy. New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
  13. Elias, N. (1978). The civilizing process: The history of manners. New York: Urizen Books.Google Scholar
  14. Elias, N. (1982). In H. Martins, R. Whitley (Eds.), Scientific establishments and hierarchies: sociology of the sciences yearbook. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
  15. Franks, D. (1989). Alternatives to Collin’s use of emotion in the theory of ritualistic chains. In Symbolic interaction, Vol. 12, pp. 97–101. Berkeley: Caliber.Google Scholar
  16. Franks, D. and V. Gecas (1992) (Eds.) Social perspectives on emotion, Vol. 1, pp. 25–46. Greenwich: JAI Press.Google Scholar
  17. Gazzaniga, M. S. (1985). The social brain. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  18. Gazzaniga, M. S. (1998). The mind’s past. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  19. Geertz, C. (1973). The interpretation of cultures. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  20. Gibbs, R. W. (2006). Embodiment and cognitive science. Cambridge; New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  21. Gillihan, S. J. and M. J. Farah (2005). Is self special? A critical review of evidence from experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego. American Psychologist, 35(7), 603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Gunsnard, D. A., E. Akbudak, G. L. Shulman and M. E. Raichle (2001). Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain functioning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 98(7), 4259–4264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Iacoboni, M. (2008). Mirroring people: The new science of how we connect with others. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
  25. LeDoux, J. E. (2002). Synaptic self: How our brains become who we are. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
  26. Lewis, M. (2000). The role of cognition in emotional development. In G. Hatano, N. Okada, H. Tanabe (Eds.), Affective minds, pp. 119. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Health Science.Google Scholar
  27. Lieberman (2003). C. Knight, M. Studdert-Kennedy, J. R. Hurford (Eds.) The evolutionary emergence of language: Social functions and the origins of linguistic form. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2000. Journal of Linguistics, 39(3), 666–672.Google Scholar
  28. Lieberman, M. D. and N. I. Eisenberger (2004). Conflict and habit: A social cognitive neuroscience approach to the self. On Building, Defending and Regulating the Self: A Psychological Perspective, 77–102.Google Scholar
  29. McAulay, L. S. (1977). The relationship between visual impairment and personal space with electrodermal response as indication of intrusion. Austin, TX: University of Texas, Austin.Google Scholar
  30. Merleau-Ponty, M. (1945). Phenomenology of perception (C. Smith Trans., 1962). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  31. Raichle, M. E., A. M. MacLeod, A. Z. Snyder, W. J. Powers, D. A. Gunsnard and G. L. Shulman (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 98(2), 676–682.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Ramachandran, V. S. and S. Blakeslee (1998). Phantoms in the brain: Probing the mysteries of the human mind. New York: William Morrow.Google Scholar
  33. Rawls, J. (1971). A theory of justice. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  34. Reilly, K. T., C. Mercier, M. H. Schieber and A. Sirigu (2007). Persistent hand movement representations in the brains of amputees. Brain, 130(2), e66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Sampson, E. (1981). Cognitive psychology as ideology. American Psychologist, 36(7), 73–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Sampson, E. (1988). The debate on individualism: Indigenous psychologies of the individual and their role in personal and societal functioning. American Psychologist, 43(1), 15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Schacter, D. L. (2001). The seven sins of memory: How the mind forgets and remembers. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  38. Scheff, T. J. (1990). Microsociology: Discourse, emotion, and social structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  39. Scott, M. and S. Lyman (1968). Accounts. American Sociological Review, 31, 46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Smith, M. B. (1968). Competence and socialization. In J. Clauson (Ed.), Socialization and society, pp. 270. Boston: Little, Brown.Google Scholar
  41. TenHouten, W. D. (2005). Time and society. New York: SUNY Press.Google Scholar
  42. Tuan, Y. (1982). Segmented worlds and self: Group life and individual consciousness. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
  43. Uddin, L. Q., M. Iacoboni, C. Lange and J. P. Keenan (2007). The self and social cognition: The role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurons. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(4), 153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Uddin, L. Q., J. T. Kaplan, I. Molnar-Szakacs, E. Zaidel and M. Iacoboni (2005). Self-face recognition activates a frontoparietal “mirror” network in the right hemisphere: An event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage, 25(3), 926–935.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Uddin, L. Q., I. Molnar-Szakacs, E. Zaidel and M. Iacoboni (2006). rTMS to the right inferior parietal lobule disrupts self-other discrimination. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1(1), 65–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Wegner, D. (2003). The mind’s self-portrait, Vol. 1001. New York: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
  47. Westen, D. (1985). Self and society: Narcissism, collectivism, and the development of morals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  48. White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66(5), 297–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Wilson, T. D. (2002). Strangers to ourselves: Discovering the adaptive unconscious. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  50. Zimmer, C. (2005). Neuroscience: The neurobiology of the self. Scientific American, 293(5), 92–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of SociologyVirginia Commonwealth UniversityRichmondUSA

Personalised recommendations