Skip to main content

The New Unconscious: Agency and Awareness

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Neurosociology
  • 1074 Accesses

Abstract

The quote above is dated: as I write this, factory workers are likely to feel lucky to be working at all. It well could be, as the country emerges from its current financial distress, factory workers may feel dissatisfied again. But the fact remains that our current states of satisfaction are often creatures of past conditions utterly unbeknownst to us and therefore unconscious.

There is a merciful mechanism in the human mind that prevents one from knowing how unhappy one is. One only realizes it if the unhappiness passes, and then one wonders how on earth one was ever able to stand it. If the factory workers once got out of factory life for six months there would be a revolution such as the world has never seen.

[The poet W.H. Auden (1939)].

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Rather than being pushed by determinant past forces of external conditioning or driven by the unconscious tensions between the psychoanalytic “superego” and “id,” voluntaristc behavior for symbolic interactionists was seated in the self-consciousness involved in taking the role of the other toward one’s own actions. This had a strong teleological character wherein actors’ present behavior is “pulled into being” by their own desired future – that is his or her positive anticipation of the future consummation of the act. In contrast to more prevalent deterministic approaches, the self was no longer rendered epiphenomenal it was in conditioning and earlier psychoanalytic formulations, but is placed on center stage as the key to a model of agency.

  2. 2.

    I am not saying that we should deny the importance of self-consciousness, far from it. I am saying that in 2010 it cannot be considered the whole ball of wax, and symbolic interaction could both enrich and expand itself by addressing the fact of unconscious symbolic and semiotic processes.

  3. 3.

    These findings are reminiscent of Marx’s methodologically questionable “false consciousness.”

References

  • Anderson, A. and Phelps, E. (2000). Expression without recognition: Contributions of the human amygdala to emotional communication. Psychological Science, 11, 106–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, E. (1973). The denial of death. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Birdwhistell, R. (1970), Kinesics and context: Essays on body motion and communication. London: Allen Lane.

    Google Scholar 

  • Birdwhistell, R. L. (1974). Korpersprache. Stuttgart: Klett.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, R. F. (1992). Inhibitory effects of awareness on affective responding: Implications for the affect-cognition relationship. In M. Clark (Ed.), Emotion: Review of personality and social psychology (p. 235). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brothers, L. (2001). Mistaken identity. New York: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burton, R. A. (2008). On being certain: Believing you are right even when you’re not. New York: St. Martin’s Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carter, R. (1999). Mapping the mind. Berkley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crick, F. H. (1994). The astonishing hypothesis: The scientific search for the soul. New York: Simon and Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. (2003). Looking for Spinoza: Joy, sorrow and the feeling brain. New York: Harcourt, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Debner, J. A. and Jacoby, L. L. (1994). Unconscious perception: Attention, awareness and control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 304–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis, A. (2005). Why we are social animals: The high road to imitation as social glue. In S. Hurley and N. Chater (Eds.), Perspectives on imitation: From cognitive neuroscience to social science (p. 207). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman Gerald, M. (2004). Wider than the sky: The phenomenal gift of consciousness. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. and Davidson, R. J. (1994). The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franks, D. D. (2006). The neuroscience of emotions. In J. E. Stets and J. H. Turner (Eds.), Handbook of the sociology of emotions. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C. (2007). Making up the mind: How the brain creates our mental world. Malden, MA: Blackwell Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M. (1985). The social brain. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M. (1998). The mind’s past. Berkeley: University of California.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. and Banaji, M. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes and self-esteem and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 4–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenspan, S. and Shanker, S. (2004). The first Idea: How symbols, language, and intelligence evolved to from our modern humans. Cambridge MA: Cambridge Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, S. (1999). Navigating the sound stream of human social interaction. In D. Franks and T. Smith (Eds.), In mind, brains and society: Toward a neurosociology of emotion (pp. 213–246). Stamford, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, S. and Gallegher, T. (2002). Spectorial analysis of candidates’ nonverbal vocal communication. Social Psychological Quarterly, 65(30), 295–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, A. J., Whalen, P. J., Shin, L. M., McInerney, S. C., Fischer, H., and Rauch, S. L. (2000). Differential response in the human amygdala to racial outgroup vs ingroup face stimuli. Neuroreport, 11(11), 2351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, E. C. (1963). Race relations and the sociological imagination. American Sociological Review, 28(6), 879.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L., A. P. Yonellinas and J. M. Jennings (1997). The relation between consciousness and unconscious (automatic) influences: A declaration of independence. In J. D. Cohen, J. W. Schooner (Eds.) Scientific approaches to consciousness, pp. 13–48. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., M. R. Banaji and B. Nosek (2004). A decade of system justification system: Accumulated evidence of conscious and unconscious olstering of the status quo. Political Psychology, 25(6), 881–919.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katz, J. (1999). How emotions work. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kihlstrom, J. F., S. Mulvancy, B. A. Tobias and I. P. Tobias (2000). The emotional unconscious. In E. Eich (Eds.) Cognition and emotion, pp. 30–86. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. and M. Johnson (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K. (1950). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. Jeffers (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. (2002). Synaptic self: How our brains become who we are. New York: Penguin Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Libet, B. (1996). Neural time factors in conscious and unconscious mental functions. In S. R. Hameroff et al. (Eds.) Toward a science of consciousness. First discussions and debates, pp. 337–347. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcel, A. J. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: An approach to the relations between phenomenal experience and perceptual processes. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merikle, P. M., Joordens, S., and Stolz, J. A. (1995). Measuring the relative magnitude of unconscious influences. Consciousness and Cognition, 4(4), 422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mills, C. W. (1959). The sociological imagination. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E. and Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84(3), 231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osrow, J. M. (1990). Social sensitivity: A study of habit and experience. New York: SUNY Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polanyi, M. (1958). Personal knowledge: Towards a post-critical philosophy. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pyszczynski, T., S. Solomon and J. Greenberg (2002). In the wake of 9/11: The psychology of terror. Washington DC: American Psychological Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran, V. S. and S. Blakeslee (1998). Phantoms in the brain: Probing the mysteries of the human mind. New York: William Morrow.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheff Thomas, J. (1990). Microsociology: Discourse, emotion, and social structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, D. L. (2004). Why we lie: The evolutionary roots of deception and the unconscious mind. New York: St. Martin’s Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sudnow, D. (1979). Talk’s body: A meditation between two keyboards. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tavris, C. and Aronson, E. (2007). Mistakes were made (but not by me): Why we justify foolish beliefs, bad decisions, and hurtful acts (1st edn.). Orlando, FL: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wentworth, W. and J. Ryan (1992). Balancing body, mind and culture: The place of emotion in social life. In D. D. Franks, V. Gecas (Eds.) Social perspectives on emotion, vol. 1, pp. 25–46. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (2001). Closing the debate over the independence of affect. In J. P. Forgas (Eds.) Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition, pp. 31–58. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A. (1999). Distinguishing unconscious from conscious emotional processes: Methodological considerations and theoretical implications. In T. Dalgleish, M. Power (Eds.) Handbook of cognition and emotion, pp. 321–349. New York: John Wiley and Sons.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David D. Franks .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Franks, D.D. (2010). The New Unconscious: Agency and Awareness. In: Neurosociology. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-5531-9_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics