Advertisement

A Review of Models of Consciousness

  • John G. Taylor
Chapter
Part of the Springer Series in Cognitive and Neural Systems book series (SSCNS)

Abstract

We review the main models of consciousness and develop various criteria to assess them. One of the most important criteria is that of the existence of the inner self of Western phenomenology (following Hussar, Sartre, Merleau-Ponty and colleagues). Only one model survives that criterion, that of CODAM (Taylor, 2007; ibid, 2010a; ibid, 2010b). Another test is that of agreeing with relevant experimental data (to be considered in detail later), especially a few data sets of specific relevance to consciousness (Taylor and Fragopanagos, 2007). A number pass this test over a limited range of data (not necessarily the most relevant data for consciousness). Very few models pass the third test of explicitly involving attention (regarded as the gateway to consciousness). A fourth and final test is that of helping explain defects of experience in various diseases of the mind, especially schizophrenia. Only one model survives that test (again CODAM, Taylor, 2007, 2010a). Our conclusion is that although many models emphasise various components of consciousness creation, very few yet include all relevant components.

Keywords

Attentional Blink Granger Causality Work Memory Conscious Experience Infinite Regress 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Aleksander I and Morton H (2007) Phenomenology and digital neural architectures. Neural Networks 20:932–937CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Amari S-I (1977) Dynamics of pattern formation in lateral-inhibition type neural fields. Biological Cybernetics 27:77–87CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Baars B (1998) A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
  4. Baars B (2002) The conscious access hypothesis: origins and recent evidence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6(1):47–52CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Bar M and Biederman I (1998) Sublimal visual priming. Psychological Science 9(6):464–469CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Braun J (2008) Attention and awareness. In T Bayne, A Cleeremans and P Wilken. Oxford: Companion to Consciousness OxfordGoogle Scholar
  7. Baars BJ and Franklin S (2007) An architectural model of conscious and unconscious model of global brain functions: Global workspace theory and IDA Neural Networks 20(9):955–961Google Scholar
  8. Bressler SL, Tang W, Sylvester CM, Shulman GL and Corbetta M (2008) Top-Down control of human visual cortex by frontal and parietal cortex in anticipatory visual spatial attention. Journal of Neuroscience 28(40):10056–10061CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Cermolacce M, Naudin J and Parnas J (2007) The “minimal self” in psychopathology: Re-examining the self-disorders in the schizophrenia spectrum. Consciousness and Cognition 16:703–714CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Carruthers P (1996) Natural theories of consciousness European Journal of Philosophy 6:203–222Google Scholar
  11. Chalmers D (1995) The conscious mind: in search of a fundamental theory. Oxford: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  12. Corbetta M, Patel G and Shulman G (2008) The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind. Neuron 58:306–324CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Crick FHC and Koch C (1990) Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness. Seminars in the Neurosciences 2:263–275Google Scholar
  14. Crick FHC (2004) The astonishing hypothesis. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
  15. Crick FHC and Koch C (2003) A framework for consciousness. Nature neuroscience 6:119–126CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Edelman GM and Tononi G (2000) A universe of consciousness. New York: Basic BooksGoogle Scholar
  17. Edelman GM (1992) Bright air brilliant fire. New York: Basic BooksGoogle Scholar
  18. Fernandez-Duque D, Thornton JM (2000) Change detection without awareness. Visual Cognition 7:323–344CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Freeman WJ (2007) Indirect biological measures of consciousness from field studies of brains as dynamical systems. Neural Networks 20:1021–1031CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Gold I (1999) Does 40Hz oscillation play a role in visual consciousness? Consciousness & Cognition 8:186–195CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gregoriou GG, Gotts SJ, Zhou H and Desimone R (2009) High-Frequency, Long-Range Coupling Between Prefrontal and Visual Cortex During Attention. Science 324:1207–1210CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Grossberg, S (1976) Adaptive pattern classification and universal recoding, I: Parallel development and coding of neural feature detectors & II: Feedback, expectation, olfaction, and illusions. Biological Cybernetics 23:121–134 & 187–202Google Scholar
  23. Kentridge RW, Heywood CA and Weiskrantz L (1999) Attention without awareness in blindsight. Proceedings of Biological Sciences 266:1805–1811CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Koch C and Tsuchiya N (2007) Attention and consciousness: two distinct brain processes Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11(1):16–22Google Scholar
  25. Kounios et al (2009) The origin of insight in resting state behaviour. Neuropsychologia 46(1):281–291CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Laberge D and Kasevich R (2007) The apical dendritic theory of consciousness. Neural Networks 20:1004–1020CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Lamme VAF (2006) Towards a true neural stance on consciousness Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10(11):494–501Google Scholar
  28. Landman R, Spekreijse H and Lamme VAF (2003) Large capacity storage of integrated objects before change blindness. Vision Research 43:149–164CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Levine J (1983) Materialism and qualia: the explanatory gap. Pacific Philosophical Quarterly 64:354–361Google Scholar
  30. Nagel T (1974) What is it like to be a bat? Philosophical Review 83:434–450CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Pastukhov A and Braun J (2005) Perceptual reversals need no prompting from attention. Journal of Vision 5:1–13Google Scholar
  32. Pollen DA (2003) Explicit neural representations, recursive neural networks and conscious visual perception. Cerebral Cortex 13(8):807–814.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Reddy L, Wilken P and Koch P (2004) Face-gender discrimination is possible in the near-absence of attention. Journal of Vision 4:106–117CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Rolls ET (2007) A computational neuroscience approach to consciousness. Neural Networks 20:962–982CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Rosenthal D (1986) Two concepts of consciousness. Philosophical Studies 49:329–359CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Rosenthal D (1993) Thinking that one thinks. In G Humphreys and M Davies (eds) Consciousness. Oxford: BlackwellGoogle Scholar
  37. Rowlands M (2001) Consciousness and higher-order thoughts. Mind & Language 16(3):290–310CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Sass LA and Parnas J (2003) Schizophrenia, consciousness and the self. Schizophr Bull 29(3):427–444PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Seth A (2007) Models of consciousness. Scholarpedia 2(1):1328Google Scholar
  40. Seth A, Izhikevich E, Reeke GN and Edelman GM (2006) Theories and measures of consciousness: An extended framework. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA 103(28):10799–10804CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Shanahan MP (2005) Global access, embodiment, and the conscious subject. Journal of Consciousness Studies 12(12):46–66Google Scholar
  42. Shapiro KL, Raymond JE and Ansell KM (1994) Attention to visual pattern information produces the attentional blink in rapid serial visual presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance 20:357–371CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Shoemaker S (1968) Self Reference & Self-Awareness, Journal of Philosophy 65:555–567CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Sokolowski R (2000) Introduction to phenomenology. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
  45. Taylor JG, Horwitz B, Shah NJ, Fellenz WA, Mueller-Gaertner H-W and Krause JB (2000) Decomposing memory: functional assignments and brain traffic in paired word associate learning. Neural Networks 13(8–9):923–940CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Taylor JG and Fragopanagos N (2007) Resolving some confusions over attention and consciousness. Neural Networks 20:993–1003CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Taylor JG (2007) The CODAM model: Through attention to consciousness. Scholarpedia 2(11):1598CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Taylor JG (2006) The mind: a user’s manual. Chichester: WileyGoogle Scholar
  49. Taylor JG (1999a) The race for consciousness Cambridge, MA: MITGoogle Scholar
  50. Taylor JG (1999b) Neural bubble dynamics in two dimensions: foundations. Biological Cybernetics 80:5167–5174CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Taylor JG (2010a) A neural model of the loss of self in Schizophrenia Bulletin (in press)Google Scholar
  52. Taylor JG (2010b) The “I” s eye view of itself. Journal of Consciousness Studies 17(1/2), Jan–Feb, 2010Google Scholar
  53. Tononi G (2008) Consciousness as integrated information: a provisional manifesto. Biological Bulletin 215:216–242CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Tsuchiya N and Koch C (2010) Attention and consciousness/opposite effects Scholarpedia 5(1):4173Google Scholar
  55. Wallace R (2005) Consciousness: A mathematical treatment of the global neuronal workspace model. Berlin & New York: SpringerGoogle Scholar
  56. Zahavi D (2005) Subjectivity and selfhood: investigating the first-person perspective Cambridge, MA: MITGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of MathematicsKing’s CollegeLondonUK

Personalised recommendations