Advertisement

Explorations in Juror Emotion and Juror Judgment

  • Norbert L. Kerr
Chapter
Part of the Nebraska Symposium on Motivation book series (NSM, volume 56)

Abstract

The fictional sleuth, Sherlock Holmes, was acutely sensitive to the importance – even the primacy – of empirical evidence for developing workable theories. It is an insight which modern behavioral scientists might be well advised to recognize. Elsewhere (Kerr 1998), I reported some survey data that indicated that modern editors, reviewers and readers expect nearly any sound piece of behavioral science to begin with an explicit hypothesis, derived from a priori theory. I went on to suggest that this strong preference was based on both a healthy and unhealthy premise. The healthy premise is that cogent a priori theory can do much to justify, organize, and empower our observations – an axiom of the classic hypothetico-deductive model of science (e.g., Hempel 1966). The unhealthy premise is that this is always or invariably the case – that “…it is a capital mistake not to theorize, regardless of the knowledge, understanding, or even existence of the facts” (Kerr 1998, p. 201).1 This tempts us to attempt to make theoretical bricks, even when we lack empirical clay.

Keywords

Mock Juror Jury Verdict Guilty Defendant Emotional Bias Heinous Crime 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Notes

Acknowledgments

Support for preparation of this chapter was provided by the National Science Foundation (Grants SES-0214428 and BCS 9974664).

References

  1. Abramson J (2000) We, the Jury: The Jury System and the Ideal of Democracy. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  2. Amar AR (1998) The bill of rights: creation and reconstruction. Yale University Press, New Haven, CTGoogle Scholar
  3. Baron RM, Kenny DA (1986) The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 51:1173–1182PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Barrett LF, Gross J, Christensen TC, Benvenuto M (2001) Knowing what you’re feeling and knowing what to do about it: mapping the relation between emotion differentiation and emotion regulation. Cogn Emotion 15:713–724Google Scholar
  5. Baumeister RF, Vohs KD, DeWall CN, Zhang L (2007) How emotion shapes behavior: feedback, anticipation, and reflection, rather than direct causation. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 11(2): 167–203PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Bell BE, Loftus EF (1985) Vivid persuasion in the courtroom. J Pers Assess 49(6):659–664PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Berkowitz L (2003) Affect, aggression, and antisocial behavior. In: Davidson RJ et al (eds) Handbook of affective sciences. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 804–823Google Scholar
  8. Blackstone W (1979) Commentaries on the law of England. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  9. Blaney P (1986) Affect and memory: a review. Psychol Bull 99:229–246PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Bodenhausen GV, Sheppard LA, Kramer GP (1994) Negative affect and social judgment: the differential impact of anger and sadness. Eur J Soc Psychol Spec Issue 24(1):45–62Google Scholar
  11. Bornstein BH (1999) The ecological validity of jury simulations: is the jury still out? Law Hum Behav Spec Issue 23(1):75–91Google Scholar
  12. Bornstein BH, Nemeth RJ (1999) Jurors’ perception of violence: a framework for inquiry. Aggress Violent Behav 4(1):77–92Google Scholar
  13. Bowers William J, Pierce Glenn L (1980) Arbitrariness and discrimination under post-Furman capital statutes. Crime Delinquency 26(4):563Google Scholar
  14. Bright DA, Goodman-Delahunty J (2004) The influence of gruesome verbal evidence on mock juror verdicts. Psychiatry Psychol Law 11(1):154–166Google Scholar
  15. Bright DA, Goodman-Delahunty J (2006) Gruesome evidence and emotion: anger, blame, and jury decision-making. Law Hum Behav 30:183–202PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Brown DK (1997) Jury nullification within the rule of law. Minn Law Rev 81:1149–1200Google Scholar
  17. Brown R, Kulik J (1977) Flashbulb memories. Cognition 5:73–99Google Scholar
  18. Burke A, Heuer F, Reisberg D (1992) Remembering emotional events. Memory Cogn 20:277–290Google Scholar
  19. Butler P (1995) Racially based jury nullification: black power in the criminal justice system. Yale Law J 105:677–725Google Scholar
  20. Cannon WB (1929) Bodily changes in pain, hunger, fear, and rage. Branford, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  21. Carnagey NL, Anderson CA, Bushman BJ (2007) The effect of video game violence on physiological desensitization to real-life violence. J Exp Soc Psychol 43:489–496Google Scholar
  22. Carroll J, Kerr NL, Alfini J, Weaver F, MacCoun R, Feldman V (1986) Free press and fair trial: the role of behavioral research. Law Hum Behav 10:187–202Google Scholar
  23. Caught in the headlines (1981, June) Time, p 23Google Scholar
  24. Chaiken S, Trope Y (eds) (1999) Dual-process theories in social psychology. Guilford, New York, NYGoogle Scholar
  25. Chew J (1999) The need to convict: effects of crime heinousness and evidence strength on juror vilification. Unpublished Master’s Thesis. University of New South WalesGoogle Scholar
  26. Cowan CL, Thompson WC, Ellsworth PC (1984) The effects of death qualification on jurors’ predisposition to convict and on the quality of deliberation. Law Hum Behav 8:53–79Google Scholar
  27. Dann BM (1996) The constitutional and ethical implications of “must-find-the-defendant-guilty” jury instructions. In: Kleinig J, Levine J (eds) Jury ethics. Paradigm, Herndon, VA, pp 93–117Google Scholar
  28. Davis JH (1973) Group decision and social interaction: a theory of social decision schemes. Psychol Rev 80(2):97–125Google Scholar
  29. Davis JH, Kerr NL, Atkin R, Holt R, Meek D (1975) The decision processes of 6- and 12-person mock juries assigned unanimous and 2/3 majority rules. J Pers Soc Psychol 32:1–14Google Scholar
  30. Douglas KS, Lyon DR, Ogloff JRP (1997) The impact of graphic photographic evidence on mock juror decisions in a murder trial: probative or prejudicial. Law Hum Behav 21:485–501PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Doyle AC (2003) The complete Sherlock Holmes, vol 1. Spark Educational Publishing, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  32. Erber R, Wegner DM, Therriault N (1996) On being cool and collected: mood regulation in anticipation of social interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol 70(4):757–766PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Feigenson N, Dunn MA (2003) New visual technologies in court: directions for research. Law Hum Behav 27:109–126PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Feigenson N, Park J (2006) Emotions and attributions of legal responsibility and blame: a research review. Law Hum Behav 30:143–161PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Finkel NJ, Duff KB (1991) Felony-murder and community sentiment: testing the supreme court’s assertions. Law Hum Behav 15:405–429Google Scholar
  36. Finkel NJ, Hughes KC, Smith SF, Hurabiell ML (1994) Killing kids: the juvenile death penalty and community sentiment. Behav Sci Law 12:5–20Google Scholar
  37. Forgas JP (1995) Mood and judgment: the affect infusion model (AIM). Psychol Bull 117:39–66PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Forgas JP (2006) Affect in social thinking and behavior. Psychology Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  39. Forgas JP (2008) Affect and cognition. Perspect Psychol Sci 3:94–101Google Scholar
  40. Fried M, Kaplan KJ, Klein KW (1975) Juror selection: an analysis of voir dire. In: Simon RJ (ed) The jury system in America. Sage, Beverly Hills, CA, pp 49–66Google Scholar
  41. Frontline: Inside the Jury Room (PBS television broadcast, April 8, 1986)Google Scholar
  42. Galliher JF, McCartney JL, Baum BE (1974) Nebraska’s marijuana law: a case of unexpected legislative innovation. Law Soc Rev 8:441–455Google Scholar
  43. Geimer WS, Amsterdam J (1988) Why jurors vote life or death: operative factors in ten Florida death penalty cases. Am J Crim Law 15:1–54Google Scholar
  44. Green TA (1985) Verdicts according to conscience: perspectives on the English criminal trial jury, 1200–1800. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  45. Greene E (1990) Media effects on jurors. Law Hum Behav 14:439–450Google Scholar
  46. Haidt J (2001) The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychol Rev 108:814–834PubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Hempel CG (1966) Philosophy of natural science. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJGoogle Scholar
  48. Hendrick C, Shaffer DR (1975) Murder: effects of number of killers and victim mutilation on simulated jurors’ judgments. Bull Psychon Soc 6:313–316Google Scholar
  49. Hertel G, Neuhof J, Theuer T, Kerr NL (2000) Mood effects on cooperation in small groups: does positive mood simply lead to more cooperation? Cogn Emotion 14:441–472Google Scholar
  50. Hester RK, Smith RE (1973) Effects of a mandatory death penalty on the decisions of simulated jurors as a function of heinousness of the crime. J Crim Justice 1:319–326Google Scholar
  51. Hill EL, Pfeifer JE (1992) Nullification instructions an juror guilt ratings: an examination of modern racism. Contemp Soc Psychol 16:6–10Google Scholar
  52. Hoiberg BC, Stires LK (1973) The effect of several types of pretrial publicity on the guilt attributions of simulated jurors. J Appl Soc Psychol 3:267–275Google Scholar
  53. Horowitz IA (1985) The effect of jury nullification instructions on verdicts and jury functioning in criminal trials. Law Hum Behav 9:25–36Google Scholar
  54. Horowitz IA (1988) Jury nullification: the impact of judicial instructions, arguments, and challenges on jury decision making. Law Hum Behav 12:439–454Google Scholar
  55. Horowitz IA, Kerr NL, Niedermeier KE (2001) Jury nullification: legal and psychological perspectives. Brooklyn Law Rev 66(4):1207–1249Google Scholar
  56. Horowitz IA, Kerr NL, Park ES, Gockel C (2006) Chaos in the courtroom reconsidered: emotional bias and juror nullification. Law Hum Behav 30:163–181PubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. James W (1890) The principles of psychology, vol 2. Holt, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  58. Jones A (2003) Let the verdict fit the crime: the biasing effects of crime heinousness on juror verdicts. Unpublished Honour’s Thesis, Macquarie UniversityGoogle Scholar
  59. Kaplan MF, Miller LE (1978) Reducing the effects of juror bias. J Pers Soc Psychol 36:1443–1455Google Scholar
  60. Kassin SM, Garfield DA (1991) Blood and guts: general and trial-specific effects of videotaped crime scenes on mock jurors. J Appl Soc Psychol 21:1459–1472Google Scholar
  61. Kelley HH, Holmes JG, Kerr NL, Reis HT, Rusbult CE, Lange V, Paul AM (2003) An atlas of interpersonal situations. Cambridge University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  62. Kerr NL (1978) Severity of penalty and mock jurors’ verdicts. J Pers Soc Psychol 36:1431–1442Google Scholar
  63. Kerr NL (1998) HARKing (Hypothesizing After the Results are Known). Pers Soc Psychol Rev 2:196–217PubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Kerr NL (in preparation) Heinousness of the crime and juror verdicts: moderating roles of gender and mode of presentationGoogle Scholar
  65. Kerr NL, MacCoun R (1985) The effects of jury size and polling method on the process and product of jury deliberation. J Pers Soc Psychol 48:349–363PubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Kerr NL, Atkin R, Stasser G, Meek D, Holt R, Davis JH (1976) Guilt beyond a reasonable doubt: effects of concept definition and assigned decision rule on the judgments of mock jurors. J Pers Soc Psychol 34:282–294Google Scholar
  67. Kerr NL, Kramer GP, Carroll JS, Alfini J (1991) On the effectiveness of voir dire in criminal cases with prejudicial pretrial publicity: an empirical study. Am Univ Law Rev 40:665–701Google Scholar
  68. Kerr NL, MacCoun R, Kramer GP (1996) Bias in judgment: comparing individuals and groups. Psychol Rev 103:687–719Google Scholar
  69. Kerr NL, Niedermeier K, Kaplan M (1999) Bias in jurors vs. juries: new evidence from the SDS perspective. Organ Behav Hum Dec Processes 80:70–86Google Scholar
  70. Kerr NL, Boster F, Callen CR, Braz ME, O’Brien B, Horowitz I (2008) Jury nullification instructions as amplifiers of bias. Int Comment Evidence 6(1), Article 2 (online publication at http://www.bepress.com/ice/vol6/iss1/art2/)
  71. Kerwin J, Shaffer DR (1994) Mock jurors versus mock juries: the role of deliberations in reactions to inadmissible testimony. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 20:153–162Google Scholar
  72. King NJ (1998) Silencing nullification advocacy inside the jury room and outside the courtroom. Univ Chic Law Rev 65:433–512Google Scholar
  73. Knapp A, Clark MS (1991) Some detrimental effects of negative mood on individuals’ ability to solve resource dilemmas. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 17:678–688Google Scholar
  74. Kramer GP, Kerr NL (1989) Laboratory simulation and bias in the study of juror behavior: a methodological note. Law Hum Behav 13:89–100Google Scholar
  75. Kramer GP, Kerr NL, Carroll JS (1990) Pretrial publicity, judicial remedies, and jury bias. Law Hum Behav 14:409–438Google Scholar
  76. Lazarus RS (1991) Cognition and motivation in emotion. Am Psychol 46:352–367PubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. LeDoux JE (1996) The emotional brain: the mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. Simon & Schuster, New York, NYGoogle Scholar
  78. LeDoux JE (2000) Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 23:155–184PubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Leipold A (1996) Rethinking jury nullification. VA Law Rev 82:253–324Google Scholar
  80. Lerner MJ, Miller DT (1978) Just world research and the attribution process: looking back and ahead. Psychol Bull 85:1030–1051Google Scholar
  81. Lieberman JD, Sales BD (1997) What social science teaches us about the jury instruction process. Psychol Public Policy Law 3:589–644Google Scholar
  82. Loftus EF, Loftus GR, Messo J (1987) Some facts about “weapon focus.”. Law and Human Behavior 11(1):55–62Google Scholar
  83. MacCoun RJ, Kerr NL (1988) Asymmetric influence in mock jury deliberation: jurors’ bias for leniency. J Pers Soc Psychol 54:21–33PubMedGoogle Scholar
  84. Macrae CN, Milne AB, Bodenhausen GV (1994) Stereotypes as energy-saving devices: a peek inside the cognitive toolbox. J Pers Soc Psychol 66:37–47Google Scholar
  85. Magliocca GN (1998) The philosopher’s stone: dualist democracy and the jury. Univ Colo Law Rev 69:175–212Google Scholar
  86. Marder N (1999) The myth of the nullifying jury. Northwest Univ Law Rev 93:877–923Google Scholar
  87. Mikula G, Scherer KR, Athenstaedt U (1998) The role of injustice in the elicitation of differential emotional reactions. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 24:769–783Google Scholar
  88. Neisser U (1982) Snapshots or benchmarks? In: Neisser U, Hyman IE (eds) Memory observed: remembering in natural contexts. Worth Publishers, San Francisco, pp 68–74Google Scholar
  89. Niedermeier K, Horowitz I, Kerr NL (1999) Informing jurors of their nullification power: a route to a just verdict or judicial chaos? Law Hum Behav 23:331–351Google Scholar
  90. Noble KB (1995) Ex-detective’s tapes fan racial tensions in Los Angeles. Los Angeles Times, August 31, 1995Google Scholar
  91. Nossiter A (2002) Of long memory: Mississippi and the murder of Medgar Evers. Da Capo Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  92. Oliver E, Griffitt W (1976) Emotional arousal and objective judgment. Bull Psychon Soc 8:399–400Google Scholar
  93. People v. Williams (2001) 25 Cal.4th 441 May 7, 2001. No. S066106Google Scholar
  94. Pepper DA (2000) Nullifying history: modern-day misuse of the right to decide the law. Case West Law Reserve Rev 50:599–645Google Scholar
  95. Pfeifer J, Brigham JC, Robinson T (1996) Euthanasia on trial: examining public attitudes toward non physician-assisted death. J Soc Issues 52:119–129PubMedGoogle Scholar
  96. Radzinowicz L (1948) A history of English criminal laws, vol 1. Stevens, LondonGoogle Scholar
  97. Rosen RA (1986) The “especially heinous” aggravating circumstance in capital cases – the standardless standard. N C Law Rev 64:941–992Google Scholar
  98. Rozin P, Haidt J, McCauley CR (1993) Disgust. In: Lewis M, Haviland J (eds) Handbook of emotions. Guilford, New York, pp 575–594Google Scholar
  99. Saks MJ, Marti MW (1997) A meta-analysis of the effects of jury size. Law Hum Behav 21:451–467Google Scholar
  100. Schachter S, Singer JE (1962) Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychol Rev 69:379–399PubMedGoogle Scholar
  101. Scheflin AW, Van Dyke J (1991) Merciful juries: the resilience of jury nullification. Wash Lee Law Rev 48:165–183Google Scholar
  102. Schopp RF (1996) Verdicts of conscience: nullification and necessity as jury response to crimes of conscience. S C Law Rev 69:2039–2116Google Scholar
  103. Schwarz N (2002) Situated cognition and the wisdom of feelings: cognitive tuning. In: Feldman-Barrett L, Salovey P (eds) The wisdom in feelings. Guilford, New York, pp 144–166Google Scholar
  104. Schwarz N, Clore GL (1983) Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: informative and directive functions of affective states. J Pers Soc Psychol 45:513–523Google Scholar
  105. Schwarz N, Clore GL (1996) Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In: Higgins ET, Kruglanski AW (eds) Social psychology: handbook of basic principles. Guilford, New York, pp 433–465Google Scholar
  106. Schwarz N, Clore GL (2007) Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In: Kruglanski AW, Higgins ET (eds) Social psychology: handbook of basic principles, 2nd edn. Guilford, New York, pp 385–407Google Scholar
  107. Scott PB (1989) Jury nullification: historical perspective on a modern debate. West VA Law Rev 91:389–419Google Scholar
  108. Simon RJ (1966) Murder, juries and the press. Trans-Action (May–June) 64–65Google Scholar
  109. Sparf and Hansen v. United States. 156 U.S. 5 (1895)Google Scholar
  110. St. John RR (1997) License to nullify: the democratic and constitutional deficiencies of authorized jury lawmaking. Yale Law Rev 106:2563–2597Google Scholar
  111. Stasser G (1999) A primer of social decision scheme theory: models of group influence, competitive model-testing, and prospective modeling. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 80:3–20PubMedGoogle Scholar
  112. Stasser G, Kerr NL, Davis JH (1989) Psychology of group influence. In: Paulus P (ed) Influence processes and consensus models in decision-making groups, 2nd edn. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJGoogle Scholar
  113. Strack F, Martin LL, Stepper S (1988) Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: a nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. J Pers Soc Psychol 54:768–777PubMedGoogle Scholar
  114. Strickland v. Washington. 466 U.S. 668 (1984)Google Scholar
  115. Sue S, Smith RE, Pedroza G (1975) Authoritarianism, pretrial publicity, and awareness of bias in simulated jurors. Psychol Rep 37:1299–1302PubMedGoogle Scholar
  116. Taylor SE, Klein LC, Lewis BP, Gruenewald TL, Gurung R, Updegraff JA (2000) Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: Tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychol Rev 107(3):411–429Google Scholar
  117. Thomas E, Hogue A (1976) Apparent weight of evidence, decision criteria, and confidence ratings in juror decision making. Psychol Rev 83:442–465Google Scholar
  118. Thompson CM, Dennison S (2004) Graphic evidence of violence: the impact on juror decision-making, the influence of judicial instructions and the effect of juror biases. Psychiatry Psychol Law 11:323–337Google Scholar
  119. Tiedens LZ, Linton S (2001) Judgment under emotional uncertainty: the effects of specific emotions on information processing. J Pers Soc Psychol 81:973–988PubMedGoogle Scholar
  120. Tindale RS, Smith CM, Thomas LS, Filkins J (1996) Shared representations and asymmetric social influence processes in small groups. In: Witte EH, Davis JH (eds) Understanding group behavior, vol 1: Consensual action by small groups, pp 81–103, Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum. Google Scholar
  121. Tobias JJ (1967) Crime and industrial society in the 19th century. Schocken, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  122. U.S. v. Thomas. No. 95-1337 (2nd Cir. 5-20-97)Google Scholar
  123. United States v. Battiste. C.C.D.Mass (1835)Google Scholar
  124. United States v. Dougherty. 473 F. 2d 1113 (D.C.Cir. 1972)Google Scholar
  125. United States v. Moylan. 417 F.2d 1002 (4th Cir.1969)Google Scholar
  126. Van Dyke J (1970) The jury as a political institution. Catholic Law Rev 16:224–270Google Scholar
  127. Walsh JE (2007) Three strikes laws. Greenwood Press, Westport, CTGoogle Scholar
  128. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A (1988) Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol 54:1063–1070PubMedGoogle Scholar
  129. Whalen DH, Blanchard FA (1982) Effects of photographic evidence on mock juror judgment. J Appl Soc Psychol 12:30–41Google Scholar
  130. White LT (1987) Juror decision making in the capital penalty trial. An analysis of crimes and defense strategies. Law and Hum Behav 11:113–130Google Scholar
  131. Wiener RL, Bornstein BH, Voss A (2006) Emotion and the law: a framework for inquiry. Law Hum Behav 30:231–248PubMedGoogle Scholar
  132. Zajonc RB (1980) Feeling and thinking: preferences need no inferences. Am Psychol 35(2):151–175Google Scholar
  133. Zajonc RB (1984) On the primacy of affect. Am Psychol 39:117–123Google Scholar
  134. Zajonc RB (1998) Emotions. In: Gilbert DT, Fiske ST, Lindzey G (eds) The handbook of social psychology, vols 1 and 2, 4th edn. McGraw-Hill, New York, NY, pp 591–632Google Scholar
  135. Zajonc RB, Murphy ST, Inglehart M (1989) Feeling and facial efference: implications of the vascular theory of emotion. Psychol Rev 96:395–416PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Michigan State UniversityEast LansingUSA

Personalised recommendations