We Share, Therefore We Think

  • R. Peter Hobson

Keywords

Posit Sorting Nash Stake Blindness 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Andrews K (this volume) Critter psychology: on the possibility of nonhuman animal folk psychology.Google Scholar
  2. Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U (1985) Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition 21:37–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Barresi J, Moore C (1996) Intentional relations and social understanding. Behav Brain Sci 19:107–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bosch G. (1970) Infantile autism. Transl. by Jordan D, Jordan I. Springer, New York.Google Scholar
  5. Carpenter M, Nagell K, Tomasello M (1998) Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev Serial no. 255 63:4.Google Scholar
  6. Charman T, Baron-Cohen S (1994) Another look at imitation in autism. Dev Psychopathol 6:403–413.Google Scholar
  7. Charman T, Swettenham J, Baron-Cohen S, Cox A, Baird G, Drew A (1997) Infants with autism: an investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation. Dev Psychol 33:781–789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cohen DJ (1980) The pathology of the self in primary childhood autism and Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Psychiatr Clin North Am 3:383–402.Google Scholar
  9. Dawson G, Adams A (1984) Imitation and social responsiveness in autistic children. J Abnorm Child Psychol 12:209–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. DeMyer MK (1975) The nature of the neuropsychological disability in autistic children. J Autism Child Schizophr 5:109–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Field TM, Woodson R, Greenberg R, Cohen D (1982) Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by neonates. Science 218:179–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Gallagher S (2007) Logical and phenomenological arguments against simulation theory. In: Hutto D, Ratcliffe M (eds) Folk psychology re-assessed. pp 63–78.Google Scholar
  13. Gopnik A, Meltzoff A (1997) Words, thoughts, and theories. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  14. Hamlyn DW (1974) Person-perception and our understanding of others. In: Mischel T (ed) Understanding other persons. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 1–36.Google Scholar
  15. Hamlyn DW (1978) Experience and the growth of understanding. Routledge & Kegan Paul, London.Google Scholar
  16. Hobson RP (1989) On sharing experiences. Dev Psychopathol 1:197–203.Google Scholar
  17. Hobson RP (1990) On the origins of self and the case of autism. Dev Psychopathol 2:163–181.Google Scholar
  18. Hobson RP (1991) Against the theory of ‘Theory of Mind’. Br J Dev Psychol 9:33–51.Google Scholar
  19. Hobson RP (1993a) Autism and the development of mind. Erlbaum, Hove, Sussex.Google Scholar
  20. Hobson RP (1993b) The emotional origins of social understanding. Philos Psychol 6:227–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hobson RP (2000) The grounding of symbols: a social-developmental account. In: Mitchell P, Riggs KJ (eds) Reasoning and the mind. Psychology Press, Hove, UK, pp 11–35.Google Scholar
  22. Hobson RP (2002) The cradle of thought. Macmillan, London.Google Scholar
  23. Hobson RP (2005a) Social engagement and understanding in chimpanzees and humans. Commentary on Tomasello M, Carpenter M. The emergence of social cognition in three young chimpanzees. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev, Serial no. 279 70:133–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hobson RP (2005b) Why connect? On the relation between blindness and autism. In: Pring L (ed) Autism and blindness. Whurr, London, pp 10–25.Google Scholar
  25. Hobson RP, Lee A (1998) Hello and goodbye: a study of social engagement in autism. J Autism Dev Disord 28:117–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hobson RP, Lee A (1999) Imitation and identification in autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 40:649–659.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hobson RP, Meyer J (2005) Foundations for self and other: A study in autism. Dev Sci 8:481–491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hobson RP, Brown R, Minter M, Lee A (1997) ‘Autism’ revisited: the case of congenital blindness. In: Lewis V, Collis GM (eds), Blindness and psychological development in young children. British Psychological Society, Leicester, UK, pp 99–115.Google Scholar
  29. Hornik R, Risenhoover N, Gunnar M (1987) The effects of maternal positive, neutral and negative affective communications on infant responses to new toys. Child Dev 58:937–944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hutto DD (2002) The world is not enough: shared emotions and other minds. In: Goldie P (ed) Understanding emotions: mind and morals. Ashgate, Burlington, VT.Google Scholar
  31. Hutto DD (2003) Folk psychological narratives and the case of autism. Philos Pap 32:345–361.Google Scholar
  32. Hutto DD (2004) The limits of spectatorial folk psychology. Mind Lang 19:548–573.Google Scholar
  33. Jennings WB (1973) A study of the preference for affective cues in autistic children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Memphis State University.Google Scholar
  34. Kanner L (1943) Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nerv Child 2:217–250.Google Scholar
  35. Kaye K (1982) The mental and social life of babies. Methuen, London.Google Scholar
  36. Laplanche J, Pontalis J-B (1973) The language of psycho-analysis. Hogarth, London.Google Scholar
  37. Leslie AM (1987) Pretense and representation: the origins of “theory of mind”. Psychol Rev 94:412–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Meltzoff AN, Moore MK (1977) Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198:75–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Meyer JA, Hobson RP (2004) Orientation in relation to self and other: the case of autism. Interact Stud 5:221–244.Google Scholar
  40. Meyer JA, Hobson RP (2005) Identification: the missing link between joint attention and autism? Dev Psychol (in press).Google Scholar
  41. Mundy P, Sigman M (1989) The theoretical implication of joint-attention deficits in autism. Dev Psychopathol 1:173–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Mundy P, Sigman M, Ungerer J, Sherman T (1986) Defining the social deficits of autism: the contribution of non-verbal communication measures. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 27:657–669.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Nadel J, Guerini C, Peze A, Rivet C (1999) The evolving nature of imitation as a format for communication. In: Nadel J, Butterworth G (eds) Imitation in infancy. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 209–234.Google Scholar
  44. Ohta M (1987) Cognitive disorders of infantile autism: a study employing the WISC, spatial relationship conceptualization, and gesture imitations. J Autism Dev Disord 17:45–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Piaget J (1972) The principles of genetic epistemology. Transl. by Mays W. Routledge & Kegan Paul, London.Google Scholar
  46. Scheler M (1954) The nature of sympathy. Transl. by Heath P. Routledge & Kegan Paul, London.Google Scholar
  47. Sigman M, Mundy P, Sherman T, Ungerer JA (1986) Social interactions of autistic, mentally retarded and normal children and their caregivers. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 27:647–656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Sigman MD, Kasari C, Kwon JH, Yirmiya N (1992) Responses to the negative emotions of others by autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Child Dev 63:796–807.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Smith IM, Bryson SE (1994) Imitation and action in autism: a critical review. Psychol Bull 116:259–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Smith IM, Bryson SE (1998) Gesture imitation in autism I: nonsymbolic postures and sequences. Cogn Neuropsychol 15:747–770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Sorce JF, Emde RN, Campos J, Klinnert MD (1985) Maternal emotional signaling: its effect on the visual cliff behavior of 1-year-olds. Dev Psychol 21:195–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Stawarska B (2007). Persons, pronouns, and perspectives: linguistic and developmental contributions to dialogical phenomenology. In: Hutto D, Ratcliffe M (eds) Folk psychology re-assessed. pp 79–99.Google Scholar
  53. Strawson PF (1962, originally 1958) Persons. In: Chappell VC (ed) The philosophy of mind. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ, pp 127–146.Google Scholar
  54. Trevarthen C, Hubley P (1978) Secondary intersubjectivity: confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In: Lock A (ed) Action, gesture and symbol: the emergence of language. Academic Press, London, pp 183–229.Google Scholar
  55. Tronick, E, Als H, Adamson L, Wise S, Brazelton TB (1978) The infant’s response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 17:1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Vygotsky LS (1962) Thought and language. Transl. by Hanfmann E, Vakar G. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  57. Walden TA, Ogan TA (1988) The development of social referencing. Child Dev 59:1230–1240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Weeks SJ, Hobson RP (1987) The salience of facial expression for autistic children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 28:137–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Werner H, Kaplan B (1963/1984) Symbol formation. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.Google Scholar
  60. Whiten A, Brown J (1998) Imitation and the reading of other minds: perspectives from the study of autism, normal children, and non-human primates. In: Braten S (ed) Intersubjective communication and emotion in ontogeny. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 260–280.Google Scholar
  61. Wimpory DC, Hobson RP, Williams JMG, Nash S (2000) Are infants with autism socially engaged? A study of recent retrospective parental reports. J Autism Dev Disord 30:525–536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Wittgenstein L (1958) Philosophical investigations. Transl. by Anscombe GEM. Blackwell, Oxford.Google Scholar
  63. Zahavi D (2007) Expression and empathy. In: Hutto D, Ratcliffe M (eds) Folk psychology re-assessed pp 25–40.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© springer 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • R. Peter Hobson

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations