Advertisement

Intelligence and Hereditary Genius

  • H. J. Eysenck
Part of the Studies in Biology, Economy and Society book series (SBES)

Abstract

Ever since Galton raised the question of ‘hereditary genius’ (Galton, 1869), we have been plagued by the problem of distinguishing between high intelligence and the creativity that is the prerogative of genius. Terman (1925) entitled his study of children with IQs above 140 ‘Genetic Studies of Genius’, ensuring the identification of IQ and genius, but while historical geniuses have usually been rated high in IQ (Cox, 1926), the reverse is certainly not true: there are many highly intelligent people with IQs above 180 whom nobody would call geniuses. Empirical studies of creativity, or ratings of creativity, have usually shown only slight correlation with IQ, suggesting independence between very high IQ and creativity (Prentky, 1980). IQ may be a necessary, but is certainly not a sufficient condition of ‘genius’; creativity clearly implies much more than intelligence.

Keywords

Neural Element Creative People Creative Person Creative Thought Uniqueness Score 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Amabile, T. M. (1983) The Social Psychology of Creativity (New York: Springer).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Andreani, O. and S. Orin (1972) Le radici psicologiche del Talento (Bologna: Societa Editrice Il Mulino).Google Scholar
  3. Barron, F. (1963) Creativity of Psychological Health (London: Van Nos-trand).Google Scholar
  4. Barron, F. (1969) The Creative Person and the Creative Process (New York: Holt).Google Scholar
  5. Cameron, N. (1947) The Psychology of Behaviour Disorders (Boston: Houghton Mifflin).Google Scholar
  6. Campbell, D. T. (1960) ‘Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes’, Psychol. Rev., vol. 67, pp. 380–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Canter, S. (1973) ‘Some aspects of cognitive function in twins’, in G. S. Claridge, S. Canter and W. I. Hume (eds) Personality Differences and Biological Variation: A Study of Twins. (Oxford: Pergamon).Google Scholar
  8. Cattell, R. B. and J. Butcher (1968) The Prediction of Achievement and Creativity (New York: Bobbs-Merril).Google Scholar
  9. Cattell, R. B. and J. G. A. Drevdahl (1955) ‘Comparison of the personality profile (16 PF) of eminent researchers with that of eminent teachers and administrators and of the general public’, British Journal of Psychology, vol. 46, pp. 248–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Claridge, G. (1981) ‘Psychoticism’, in R. Lynn (ed.) Dimensional Personality (Oxford: Pergamon Press).Google Scholar
  11. Claridge, G. (1983) ‘The Eysenck psychoticism scale’, in J. N. Butcher and C. D. Spielberger (eds) Advances in Personality Assessment, vol. 2, pp. 71–114 (Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum).Google Scholar
  12. Cox, C. M. (1926) ‘The Early Mental Traits of Three Hundred Geniuses’, Genetic Studies of Genius, vol. 2 (Stanford University Press).Google Scholar
  13. Crutchfield, R. S. (1962) ‘Conformity and creative thinking’, in H. E. Gruber, G. Terrell and M. Werkheimer (eds) Contemporary Approaches to Creative Thinking (New York: Etherton Press).Google Scholar
  14. Csikszentmihalyi, M. and J. W. Getzels (1973) ‘The personality of young artists: an empirical and theoretical exploration’, British Journal of Psychology, vol. 64, pp. 91–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Drevdahl, J. E. (1956) ‘Factors of importance for creativity’, Journal of Clinical Psychology, vol. 12, pp. 21–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Eaves, L., H. J. Eysenck and N. Martin (1989) Genes, Culture and Personality: An Empirical Approach (New York: Academic Press).Google Scholar
  17. Eysenck, H. J. (1972) ‘Personal preferences, aesthetic sensitivity and personality in trained and untrained subjects’, Journal of Personality, vol. 40, pp. 544–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Eysenck, H. J. (1978) ‘Personality and learning’, in S. Murray-Smith (ed.), Melbourne Studies in Education (Melbourne University Press) pp. 134–81.Google Scholar
  19. Eysenck, H. J. (1979) The Structure and Measurement of Intelligence (New York: Springer).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Eysenck, H. J. and M. Castle (1970) ‘Training in art as a factor in the determination of preference judgements for polygons’, British Journal of Psychology, vol. 61, pp. 65–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Eysenck, H. J. and M. W. Eysenck (1985) Personality and Individual Differences: A natural science approach (New York: Plenum Press).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Eysenck, H. J. and S. B. G. Eysenck (1975) Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (San Diego: Edits).Google Scholar
  23. Eysenck, H. J. and S. B. G. Eysenck (1976) Psychoticism as a Dimension of Personality (London: Hodder & Stoughton).Google Scholar
  24. Furneaux, W. D. (1960) ‘Intellectual abilities and problem-solving behaviour’, in H. J. Eysenck (ed.) Handbook of Abnormal Psychology (London: Pitman) pp. 167–92.Google Scholar
  25. Galton, F. (1869) Hereditary Genius (London: Macmillan).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Getzels, J. W. and P. W. Jackson (1962) Creativity and Inteland Intelligence (New York: Wiley).Google Scholar
  27. Götz, K. O. and K. Götz (1973) ‘Introversion-extraversion and neuroticism in gifted and ungifted art students’, Perceptual and Motor Skills, vol. 36, pp. 675–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Götz, K. O. and K. Götz (1979a) ‘Personality characteristics of professional artists’, Perceptual and Motor Skills, vol. 49, pp. 327–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Götz, K. O. and K. Götz (1979b) ‘Personality characteristics of successful artists’, Perceptual and Motor Skills, vol. 49, pp. 919–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hammer, M. and J. Zubin (1968) ‘Evolution, culture and psychopathology’, Journal of General Psychology, vol. 78, pp. 154–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Heston, I. I. (1966) ‘Psychiatric disorders in foster home-reared children of schizophrenic mothers’, British Journal of Psychiatry, pp. 112, 819–25.Google Scholar
  32. Hudson, I. (1966) Contrary Imaginations (London: Methuen).Google Scholar
  33. Hudson, I. (1968) Frames of Mind. Ability, Perception and Self-Perception in the Arts and Sciences (London: Methuen).Google Scholar
  34. Jarvik, I. F. and S. B. Chadwick (1973) ‘Schizophrenia and survival’, in M. Hammer, K. Salzinger and S. Sutton (eds) Psychopathology (New York: Wiley).Google Scholar
  35. Karlsson, J.I. (1968) ‘Generalogic studies of schizophrenia’, in D. Rosenthal and S. S. Key (eds) The Transmission of Schizophrenia (Oxford: Pergamon).Google Scholar
  36. Karlsson, J. I. (1970) ‘Genetic association of giftedness and creativity with schizophrenia’, Heredity, vol. 66, pp. 177–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Karlsson, J. I. (1978) Inheritance of Creative Intelligence (Chicago: Nelson-Hall).Google Scholar
  38. Lange-Eichbaum, W. (1956) Geie, Irrsin and Ruhm (Munich: Ernst Reinhardt).Google Scholar
  39. Lytton, H. (1971) Creativity and Education (London: Routledge & Kegal Paul).Google Scholar
  40. MacKinnon, D. W. (1962) ‘The nature and nurture of creative talent’, American Psychologist, vol. 17, pp. 484–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. MacKinnon, D. W. et al. (1961), proceedings of the conference on ‘The Creative Person’, University of California Alumini Center, Lake Tahoe. McNeil, T. F. (1971) ‘Prebirth and postbirth influence on the relationship between creative ability and recorded mental illness’, Journal of Personality, pp. 39, 391–406.Google Scholar
  42. Payne, R. W. (1960) ‘Cognitive abnormalities’, in H. J. Eysenck (ed.) Handbook of Abnormal Psychology (London: Pitman).Google Scholar
  43. Payne, R. W. and J. H. G. Hewlett (1960) ‘Thought disorder in psychotic patients’, in H. J. Eysenck (ed.) Experiments in Personality (London: Routledge & Kegan Paul), pp. 3–104.Google Scholar
  44. Prentky, R. A. (1980) Creativity and Psychopathology (New York: Praeger).Google Scholar
  45. Roe, A. (1952) ‘A psychologist examines sixty-four eminent scientists’, Scientific American, vol. 187, pp. 21–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Roe, A. (1953) ‘A psychological study of eminent psychologists and anthropologists and a comparison with biological and physical scientists’, Psychological Monographs, vol. 67, no. 352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Simonton, K. (1988) Scientific Genius (New York: Cambridge University Press).Google Scholar
  48. Spearman, C. (1927) The Abilities of Man (London: Macmillan).Google Scholar
  49. Taylor, L. W. and F. Barron (eds) (1963) Scientific Creativity: Its Recognition and Development (New York: John Wiley).Google Scholar
  50. Terman, L. M. (1925) ‘Mental and physical traits of a thousand gifted children’, Genetic Studies of Genius (Vol. 1) (Stanford University Press).Google Scholar
  51. Torrance, E. P. (1962) Guiding Creative Talent (Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice Hall).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Wallach, M. A. and N. Kogan (1965) Modes of Thinking in Young Children (New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston).Google Scholar
  53. Wankowski, J. A. (1973) Temperament, Motivation and Academic Achievement (Birmingham: University of Birmingham Educational Survey and Counselling Unit).Google Scholar
  54. Woody, E. and G. Claridge (1977) ‘Psychoticism and thinking’, British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, vol. 16, pp. 241–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Zuckerman, M. (1989) ‘Personality in the third dimension: A psychological approach’, Personality and Individual Differences, vol. 10, pp. 391–418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The Galton Institute 1993

Authors and Affiliations

  • H. J. Eysenck

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations