Advertisement

Possible Redundance of Spinal Pathways for Behavioural Thermosensitivity

  • Ulf Norrsell
Part of the Wenner-Gren Center International Symposium Series book series (WGS)

Abstract

Studies of animal’s ability to utilize particular sensory stimuli before and after restricted lesions of the central nervous system employ a traditional method of sensory neurophysiology. The method is not accepted uncritically, however, and a certain disenchantment stems from findings of rapid postoperative recovery, which have indicated a somewhat unexpected, functional redundance of many central nervous structures. Nevertheless, we have used that method for several years at our laboratory for the purpose of locating the spinal, ascending, thermosensory pathways of the cat. The background was formed by numerous reports of thermosensory anaesthesia which had been caused by restricted lesions in the ventral half of the spinal cord of human patients, and indicated a possible lack of redundance in the distal parts of the thermosensory system. The symptoms and the lack of redundance have been attributed to interruption of a unique, thermosensory, spino-thalamic pathway (cf. Norrsell, 1979). Since thermal sensations constitute cutaneous sensory qualities, studies of the thermosensory system for above mentioned reasons might provide a profitable approach for extension of our knowledge about the somatosensory system.

Keywords

Spinal Cord Lesion Unilateral Lesion Lateral Funiculus Body Half Warm Stimulus 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Banks, W.P. (1973). Reaction time as a measure of summation of warmth. Percept. Psychophys., 13, 321–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Davison, C. and Schick, W. (1935). Spontaneous pain and other subjective sensory disturbances. A clinicopathologic study. Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., 34, 1204–1237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Finger, S. and Norrsell, U. (1974). Temperature sensitivity of the paw of the cat: a behavioural study. J. Physiol. (Lond.), 239, 631–646.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Hardy, J.D. and Oppel, T.W. (1937). Studies of temperature sensation. III. The sensitivity of the body to heat and the spatial summation of the end organ responses. J. clin. Invest., 16, 533–540.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  5. Hardy, J.D. and Oppel, T.W. (1938). Studies in temperature sensation. IV. The stimulation of cold sensation by radiation. J. clin. Invest., 17, 771–778.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  6. Hellon, R.F. and Mitchell, D. (1975). Convergence in a thermal afferent pathway in the rat. J. Physiol. (Lond.), 248, 359–376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Kumazawa, T., Perl, E.R., Burgess, P.R. and Whitehorn, D. (1975). Ascending projections from marginal zone (Lamina 1) neurons of the dorsal horn. J. comp. Neurol., 162, 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Marks, L.E. and Stevens, J.C. (1973). Spatial summation of warmth: Influence of duration and configuration of the stimulus. Am. J. Psychol., 86, 251–267.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Norrsell, U. (1974). An automatic T-maze for temperature discrimination in the cat. Physiol. Behav., 12, 297–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Norrsell, U. (1978). Testing procedures and the interpretation of behavioral data. In Recovery from brain damage. (ed. S. Finger). Plenum, New York.Google Scholar
  11. Norrsell, U. (1979). Thermosensory defects after cervical spinal cord lesions in the cat. Exp. Brain Res., 35, 479–494.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Norrsell, U. (1980a). Ipsi- and contralateral cold sensitivity after unilateral lesions of the lateral spinal funiculus of the cat. Acta physiol. scand., 109, 16A.Google Scholar
  13. Norrsell, U. (1980b). Thermosensitivity after unilateral transection of the lateral spinal funiculus of the cat. Neurosci. Letters, Suppl. 5, S479.Google Scholar
  14. Norrsell, U. (1983). Unilateral behvioural thermosensitivity after transection of the lateral funiculus in the cervical spinal cord of the cat. Exp. Brain Res., in press.Google Scholar
  15. Perl, E.R., Whitlock, D.G. and Gentry, J.R. (1962). Cutaneous projection to second-order neurons of the dorsal colulmn system. J. Neurophysiol., 25, 337–358.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Price, D.D. and Browe, A.C. (1975). Spinal cord coding of graded nonnoxious and noxious temperature increases. Exp. Neurol., 48, 201–221.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Rozsa, A.J. and Kenshalo, D.R. (1977). Bilateral spatial summation of cooling of symmetrical sites. Percept. Psychophys., 21, 455–462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Simon, E. (1972). Temperature signals from skin and spinal cord converging on spinothalamic neurons. Pflügers Arch., 337, 323–332.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Wilson, S.A. Kinnier (1927). Dysaesthesiae and their neural correlates. Brain, 50, 428–462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The Wenner-Gren Center 1984

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ulf Norrsell
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PhysiologyUniversity of GöteborgGöteborgSweden

Personalised recommendations