Skip to main content

Interfield Differences in Reaction Times to Lateralised Visual Stimuli in Normal Subjects

  • Chapter
  • 19 Accesses

Abstract

When a visual stimulus is projected to the right of a fixation point the response with the right hand to that stimulus is faster than the response with the left hand, and similarly, when a stimulus is projected to the left of fixation the response with the left hand is faster than that with the right hand. This advantage of ipsilateral over contralateral responses was first described by Poffenberger at the beginning of the century in experiments in which he measured reaction times to simple light stimuli. Poffenberger (1912) attributed this advantage (which was of about 4 ms in his experiments) to the different lengths of the nervous circuits responsible for the two responses. Thus ispsilateral responses can be executed using central nervous circuits located entirely in one hemisphere, whereas the contralateral responses need pathways which cross the interhemispheric commissures. Since the pathway in this last condition is longer and most likely includes one additional synapse, a response mediated by it requires more time. Poffenberger also calculated, using the few data available on the conduction velocity of nervous fibres, how long the delay should be between ipsilateral and contralateral responses provided his interpretation was correct. He wrote: ‘If the speed of transmission of the nerve impulse be taken as 30 m/per second, then in one sigma it would travel 3 cm. Considering the length of these fibers (corpus callosum) to be 6 cm the reaction time would be 2 sigma, which would be constant entity.’ (Poffenberger, 1912, p. 68).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Anzola, G. P., Bertoloni, G., Buchtel, H. A. and Rizzolatti, G. (1977). Spatial compatibility and anatomical factors in simple and choice reaction time. Neuropsychologia, 15, 295–302

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Arutyunova, A. S. and Blinkov, S. M. (1962). Latent period of motor reaction with hemianopsia (in Russian). J. Higher Nervous Activity, 12, 432–36

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlucchi, G. and Rizzolatti, G. (1968). Binocularly driven neurons in visual cortex of split-chiasm cats. Science, 159, 308–10

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Berlucchi, G., Gazzaniga, M. S. and Rizzolatti, G. (1967). Microelectrode analysis of transfer of visual information by the corpus callosum. Arch. Ital. Biol., 105, 583–96

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Berlucchi, G., Heron, W., Hyman, R., Rizzolatti, G. and Umiltà, C. (1971). Simple reaction times of ipsilateral and contralateral hand to lateralized visual stimuli. Brain, 94, 419–30

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Berlucchi, G., Crea, F., Di Stefano, M. and Tassinari, G. (1977). The influence of spatial stimulus-response compatibility on simple and choice reaction time of ipsilateral and contralateral hand to lateralized light stimuli. J. exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform., 3, 505–17

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brebner, J. (1973). S-R compatibility and changes in RT with practice. Acta Psychol., 37, 95–106

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brebner, J., Shephard, M. and Cairney, P. (1972). Spatial relationships and S-R compatibility. Acta Psychol., 36, 1–15

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1974). Division of function and integration of behavior. The Neurosciences, Third Study Program (F. O. Schmitt and F. G. Worden, eds.), MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, pp. 31–41

    Google Scholar 

  • Filbey, R. A. and Gazzaniga, M. S. (1969). Splitting the brain with reaction time Psychon. Sci. 17, 335–36

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitts, P. M. and Seeger, C. M. (1953). S-R compatibility: spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. J. Exp. Psychol., 46, 199–210

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M. S. (1971). Reply to McKeever and Huling. Psychon. Sci, 22, 223–24

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hubel, D. H. and Wiesel, T. N. (1967). Cortical and callosal connections concerned with the vertical meridian of visual fields in the cat. J. Neurophysiol., 30, 1561–73

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jeeves, M. A. (1969). A comparison of interhemispheric transmission times in acallosals and normals. Psychon. Sci., 16, 245–46

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jeeves, M. A. (1972). Hemisphere differences in response rates to visual stimuli in children. Psychon. Sci., 27, 200–203

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawrence, D. G. and Kuypers, H. G. J. M. (1968). The functional organization of the motor system in the monkey. Brain, 91, 1–14

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McKeever, W. F. and Gill, K. M. (1972). Interhemispheric transfer time for visual stimulus information varies as a function of the retinal locus of stimulation. Psychon. Sci., 26, 308–10

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKeever, W. F., Gill, K. M. and Van Deventer, A. D. (1975). Letter versus dot stimuli as tools for ‘splitting the normal brain with reaction time’. Q. Jl Exp. Psychol., 27, 363–73

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moscovitch, M. and Catlin, J. (1970). Interhemispheric transmission of information: measurement in normal man. Psychon. Sci., 18, 211–13

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poffenberger, A. T. (1912). Reaction time to retinal stimulation with special reference to the time lost in conduction through nerve centers. Archs Psychol., 23, 1–73

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G. and Buchtel, H. A. (1977). Hemispheric superiority in reaction time to faces: a sex difference. Cortex, 13, 300–305

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Umiltà, C. and Berlucchi, G. (1971). Opposite superiorities of the right and left cerebral hemispheres in discriminative reaction time to physiognomical and alphabetic material. Brain, 94, 431–42

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasch, J. (1954). Size, distribution, and number of fibers in human corpus callosum. Anat. Rec., 119, 119–35

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tower, S. S. (1940). Pyramidal lesion in the monkey. Brain, 63, 36–90

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Travis, A. M. (1955). Neurological deficiences after ablation of the precentral motor area in Macaca mulatta. Brain, 78, 155–73

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Umiltà, C., Rizzolatti, G., Marzi, C. A., Zamboni, G., Franzini, C., Camarda, R. and Berlucchi, G. (1974). Hemispheric differences in the discrimination of line orientation. Neuropsychologia, 12, 165–74

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wallace, R. J. (1971). S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code. J. Exp. Psychol., 88, 354–60

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Whitteridge, D. (1965). Area 18 and the vertical meridian of the visual field. Ciba Fdn Study Grps., 20, 115–20

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Copyright information

© 1979 I. Steele Russell, M. W. van Hof and G. Berlucchi

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Rizzolatti, G. (1979). Interfield Differences in Reaction Times to Lateralised Visual Stimuli in Normal Subjects. In: Russell, I.S., van Hof, M.W., Berlucchi, G. (eds) Structure and Function of Cerebral Commissures. Palgrave, London. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-349-03645-5_32

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics