Skip to main content

Superstitious Perception: Comparing Perceptual Prediction by Humans and Neural Networks

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Part of the book series: Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences ((CTBN,volume 41))

Abstract

Recent developments in convolutional neural networks (CNNs) have introduced new ways to model the complex processes of human vision. To date, the comparison of human vision and CNNs has focused on internal representations (i.e., receptive fields), with behavioral comparisons left largely unexplored. Here, we probe the influence of cognitive strategy on the similarity between CNN output and human behavior. We gave study participants a superstitious perception task (i.e., we asked them to detect an assigned target in white noise) while asking them to engage in either an active or passive attentional strategy. Previous research has shown that an active attentional strategy tends to engage central executive functions, whereas a passive strategy allows perceptual processes to unfold with limited central control. The results showed that the pattern of human responses in the superstitious perception task depended significantly on task strategy. Specifically, detecting targets superstitiously (i.e., false alarms) was correlated with evidence of a target’s presence in the passive condition, but not in the active condition.

Human data were compared to the performance of a CNN performing the same task, with the decision criterion of the CNN set to match the false alarm rates observed in the two strategy conditions of the human participants. CNN responses resembled those of human participants in the passive condition more closely than those in the active condition. This observation suggests that the CNN does a better job of mimicking human behavior when central executive functions are not engaged than when they are engaged. This, in turn, has important implications for what human participants are doing in the superstitious perception task. Namely, it implies that superstitious perception may have two important ingredients that are somewhat dissociable. First, there is the ability to detect weak signals in noise that correspond to the target image. This appears to be what participants are doing under passive strategy conditions; they allow externally generated signals to dominate their perceptual experience. Second, there is the ability to ignore the noise in favor of basing responses solely on internally generated signals. This seems to correspond more closely to what participants are doing under active strategy conditions, when attention is controlled by representations in memory. This research emphasizes the importance of modeling the full range of human responsiveness in even a simple noisy detection task.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    In order to ensure the stability of the CNN model, all model weights and biases were truncated to four decimal places as opposed to the unlimited number of significant digits allowed under normal CNN procedures. All analyses covered in this chapter were performed anew using the model with truncated weights. The changes had no influence on the various analyses but did noticeably reduce the quality of the generated CIs. The shape of the target was still present in both the liberal and conservative conditions. Nonetheless, the CIs lacked the clarity observed in the CIs generated from the model without truncated weights. These observations demonstrate the sensitivity of the CNN model to restricting the significant digits of the model weights. However, the observations do not change the overall conclusions of this work.

References

  • Brainard DH (1997) The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis 10(4):433–436

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Burnham KP, Anderson D (2002) Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. Springer, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Cichy RM, Khosla A, Pantazis D, Torralba A, Oliva A (2016) Deep neural networks predict hierarchical spatio-temporal cortical dynamics of human visual object recognition. Sci Rep 6(1):27755

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen J, Cohen P, West SG, Aiken LS (2003) Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences, 3rd edn. L. Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah

    Google Scholar 

  • DeAngelis GC, Ohzawa I, Freeman RD (1993) Spatiotemporal organization of simple-cell receptive fields in the cat’s striate cortex. II. Linearity of temporal and spatial summation. J Neurophysiol 69(4):1118–1135

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond A (2013) Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol 64(1):135–168

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dyan P, Abbott L (2001) Theoretical neuroscience: computational modeling of neural systems. Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Fyfe S, Williams C, Mason OJ, Pickup GJ (2008) Apophenia, theory of mind and schizotypy: perceiving meaning and intentionality in randomness. Cortex 44(10):1316–1325

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gosselin F, Bonnar L, Paul LK, Schyns PG (2001) “Superstitious” perceptions to depict pure internal object representations. J Vis 1:46

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gosselin F, Schyns PG (2003) Superstitious perceptions reveal properties of internal representations. Psychol Sci 14(5):505–509

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haffenden AM, Schiff KC, Goodale MA (2001) The dissociation between perception and action in the Ebbinghaus illusion: nonillusory effects of pictorial cues on grasp. Curr Biol 11(3):177–181

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hornik K, Stinchcombe M, White H (1989) Multilayer feedforward networks are universal approximators. Neural Netw 2(5):359–366

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby L, Brooks L (1984) Nonanalytic cognition: memory, perception, and concept learning. Psychol Learn Motiv 18:1–47

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones JP, Palmer LA (1987) The two-dimensional spatial structure of simple receptive fields in cat striate cortex. J Neurophysiol 58(6):1187–1211

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Khaligh-Razavi SM, Kriegeskorte N (2014) Deep supervised, but not unsupervised, models may explain IT cortical representation. PLoS Comput Biol 10(11):e1003915

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kheradpisheh SR, Ghodrati M, Ganjtabesh M, Masquelier T (2016) Humans and deep networks largely agree on which kinds of variation make object recognition harder. Front Comput Neurosci 10:92

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleiner M, Brainard D, Pelli D (2007) What’s new in psychtoolbox-3? Perception 36(14):1–16

    Google Scholar 

  • Krizhevsky A, Sutskever I, Hinton G (2012) ImageNet classification with deep convolutional neural networks. Adv Neural Inf Proces Syst 25

    Google Scholar 

  • Leibowitz H, Brislin R, Perlmutter L, Hennessy R (1969) Ponzo perspective illusion as a manifestation of space perception. Science 166(3909):1174–1176

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Liu J, Li J, Feng L, Li L, Tian J, Lee K (2014) Seeing Jesus in toast: neural and behavioral correlates of face pareidolia. Cortex 53:60–77

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcel AJ (1983) Conscious and unconscious perception: experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cogn Psychol 15(2):197–237

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Massaro DW, Anderson NH (1971) Judgmental model of the Ebbinghaus illusion. J Exp Psychol 89(1):147–151

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pelli DG (1997) The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis 10(4):437–442

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson JC, Abbott JT, Griffiths TL (2017) Adapting deep network features to capture psychological representations. In: Proceedings of the 38th conference of the cognitive science society

    Google Scholar 

  • Rieth CA, Lee K, Lui J, Tian J, Huber DE (2011) Faces in the mist: illusory face and letter detection. i-Perception 2(5):458–476

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ringach D, Sapiro G, Shapley R (1997) A subspace reverse correlation technique for the study of visual neurons. Vis Res 37(17):2455–2464

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ringach D, Shapley R (2004) Reverse correlation in neurophysiology. Cogn Sci 28(2):147–166

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rock I, Kaufman L (1962) The moon illusion. Sci Am 207(1):120–131

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shepard RN (1981) Psychological complementarity. In: Perceptual organization. Routledge, Abingdon

    Google Scholar 

  • Shermer M (2008) Patternicity: finding meaningful patterns in meaningless noise. Sci Am 299:74–75

    Google Scholar 

  • Smilek D, Enns JT, Eastwood JD, Merikle PM (2006) Relax! Cognitive strategy influences visual search. Vis Cogn 14(4–8):543–564

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Theunissen FE, David SV, Singh NC, Hsu A, Vinje WE, Gallant JL (2001) Estimating spatio-temporal receptive fields of auditory and visual neurons from their responses to natural stimuli. Network 12(3):289–316

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Van Selst M, Merikle PM (1993) Perception below the objective threshold? Conscious Cogn 2(3):194–203

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whittlesea B, Brooks L (1994) After the learning is over: factors controlling the selective application of general and particular knowledge. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 20(2):259

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yamins DLK, DiCarlo JJ (2016) Using goal-driven deep learning models to understand sensory cortex. Nat Neurosci 19(3):356–365

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Yamins DLK, Hong H, Cadieu CF, Solomon EA, Seibert D, DiCarlo JJ (2014) Performance-optimized hierarchical models predict neural responses in higher visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 111(23):8619–8624

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Yu RQ, Zhao J (2015) The persistence of the attentional bias to regularities in a changing environment. Atten Percept Psychophys 77(7):2217–2228

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhao J, Al-Aidroos N, Turk-Browne NB (2013) Attention is spontaneously biased toward regularities. Psychol Sci 24(5):667–677

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhao J, Yu RQ (2016) Statistical regularities reduce perceived numerosity. Cognition 146:217–222

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada grant (No. RGPIN-2013-36796) to J.T.E. Portions of this research were conducted as part of P. L’s MA thesis at the University of British Columbia (Aug 2017) entitled “Superstitious perception in humans and convolutional neural networks.”

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to James T. Enns .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Laflamme, P., Enns, J.T. (2018). Superstitious Perception: Comparing Perceptual Prediction by Humans and Neural Networks. In: Hodgson, T. (eds) Processes of Visuospatial Attention and Working Memory. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, vol 41. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2018_65

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics