Advertisement

Trajectory Formation in Timed Repetitive Movements

  • Ramesh Balasubramaniam
Chapter

Abstract

In skills as diverse as piano playing or swinging a racquet in tennis, movements comprise a pattern that involves going to and away from the target, anecdotally referred to as attack and release. Although all such voluntary actions involve timing, timed repetitive movements involve bringing an end effector periodically to a certain location in the workspace, in relation to a sensorimotor event. Research in this area has involved the characterization of synchronization errors, identification of sources of variability in synchronization, and determination of neural structures involved in organizing such behavior. While much is known about the timing errors made while synchronizing with respect to external beat, not much is understood about what kind of movement trajectories are needed for timing accuracy. In this chapter, I review some recent work that links the ideas from the trajectory formation literature to what we currently know about timing accuracy in repetitive movements. Additionally, I present a paradigm that offers to bring together the dynamical systems approach with the information processing accounts of movement timing.

Keywords

Movement Timing Movement Trajectory Repetitive Movement Limit Cycle Oscillator Dynamical System Approach 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Aschersleben G & Prinz W (1995). Synchronising actions with events: the role of sensory information. Percept Psychophys 57: 305–317.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Balasubramaniam R, Wing AM & Daffertshofer A (2004) Keeping with the beat: Movement trajectories contribute to movement timing. Exp Brain Res 159: 129–134.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Beek, PJ, Peper CE & Daffertshofer A. (2002) Modeling rhythmic interlimb coordination: beyond the Haken-Kelso-Bunz model. Brain Cog 48: 149–165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bracewell RM, Balasubramaniam R & Wing AM (2005) Interlimb coordination deficits in a case of cerebellar hemiataxia. Neurology 64: 751–752.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Carson RG & Riek S (1998). The influence of joint position on the dynamics of perception-action coupling. Exp Brain Res 121: 103–114.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Cheney PD, Fetz CE & Mewes K (1993) Neural mechanisms underlying corticospinal and rubrospinal control of limb movements. Prog Brain Res 87: 213–252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Daffertshofer A (1998) Effects of noise on the phase dynamics of nonlinear oscillators. Phys Rev E 58: 327–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Delignieres D, Lemoine L & Torre K (2004) Time interval production in tapping and oscillatory motion. HumMov Sci 23: 87–103.Google Scholar
  9. DeLong MR Crutcher MD & Georgopoulos AP (1985) Primate globus pallidus and subthalamic nucleus: functional organization. J Neurophysiol 53: 530–543.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Drewing K, Stenneken P, Cole J, Prinz W & Aschersleben G (2004). Timing of bimanual movements and deafferentation: implications for the role of sensory movement effects. Exp Brain Res 158: 50–57.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Flash T & Hogan N (1985) The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model. J Neurosci 5: 1688–1703.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Harris, CM & Wolpert DM (1998) Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature 394: 780–784.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gandevia SC & Burke D (1992) Does the nervous system depend on kinesthetic information to control natural limb movements? Behav. Brain Sci. 15: 614–632.Google Scholar
  14. Grill S & Hallett M (1995) Velocity sensitivity of human muscle spindle afferents and slowly adapting type II cutaneous mechanoreceptors. J Physiol 489: 593–602.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Kay BA, Saltzman EL, Kelso JAS (1991). Steady-state and perturbed rhythmical movements: A dynamical analysis. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform. 17: 183–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kelso JAS, Fuchs A, Holroyd T, Lancaster R, Cheyne D & Weinberg H (1998). Dynamic cortical activity in the human brain reveals motor equivalence. Nature. 392: 814–818.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Kelso, JAS., DelColle J & Schöner, G. (1990). Action-Perception as a pattern formation process. In M. Jeanerod (Ed.), Attention and Performance XIII, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 139–169.Google Scholar
  18. Kelso JAS (1995) Dynamic Patterns. Cambridge: MIT press.Google Scholar
  19. Matthews PBC (1981) Muscle spindles: their messages and their fusimotor supply. In: Handbook of Physiology. The Nervous System. Motor Control. Bethesda, MD.Google Scholar
  20. Nagasaki H (1991) Asymmetric velocity and acceleration profiles of human arm movements. Exp Brain Res 87: 653–661.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Pressing J (1999) The referential dynamics of cognition and action. Psych Rev 106: 714–747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Repp BH (2001) Processes underlying adaptations to tempo changes in sensorimotor synchronization. Hum Mov Sci 20: 277–312PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Riley MA & Turvey MT (2002) Variability and determinism in motor behavior. J Mot Behav 34: 99–125.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Schöner G (2002) Timing, clocks and dynamical systems. Brain Cog 46: 31–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Stevens LT (1886) On the time sense. Mind 11: 393–404.Google Scholar
  26. Spencer RM, Zelaznik HN, Diedrichsen J & Ivry RB (2003). Disrupted timing of discontinuous but not continuous movements by cerebellar lesions. Science 300: 1437–1439.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Swinnen SP (2002) Intermanual coordination: from behavioral principles to neural-network interactions. Nat Rev Neurosci. 3: 350–361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Turner RS, Grafton ST, Votaw JR, Delong MR & Hoffman JM (1998) Motor subcircuits mediating the control of movement velocity: a PET study. J Neurophysiol 80: 2162–2176.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Turvey MT (1990) Coordination. Am Psychol 48: 938–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Valbo AB & Wessberg J (1993) Organization of motor output in slow finger movements in man. J Physiol (Lond) 469: 673–691.Google Scholar
  31. Vorberg D & Wing AM (1996) Modeling variability and dependence in timing. In Heuer H & Keele SW (eds). Handbook of Perception & Action. 181–262. Academic Press, San Diego.Google Scholar
  32. Wann JP, Nimmo-Smith I & Wing AM (1988) Relation between velocity and curvature in movement: equivalence and divergence between a power law and a minimum-jerk model. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 14: 622–637.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Wing AM (2002). Voluntary timing and brain function. Brain Cog 48: 7–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Wing AM & Kristofferson AB (1973) Response delays and the timing of discrete motor responses. Percept Psychophys 14: 5–12.Google Scholar
  35. Wing AM & Beek PJ (2002) Movement timing—a tutorial. In Prinz, W and Hommel, B (Eds) Attention and Performance XIX. Oxford University Press. pp. 202–226.Google Scholar
  36. Yue GH, Liu JZ, Siemionow V (2000) Brain activation during finger flexion and extension movements. Brain Res 856: 291–300.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Zelaznik HN, Spencer RM & Ivry RB (2002). Dissociation of explicit and implicit timing in repetitive drawing and tapping movements. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 28: 575–588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ramesh Balasubramaniam
    • 1
  1. 1.Sensorimotor Neuroscience Laboratory, School of Human KineticsUniversity of OttawaCanada

Personalised recommendations