Skip to main content

Social and Intrapersonal Theories of Mind

“I Interact Therefore I Am”

  • Chapter

Part of the book series: The Springer Series on Human Exceptionality ((SSHE))

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Astington, J.W., Harris, P.L., Olson, D.R. (Eds.) Developing Theories of Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1988.

    Google Scholar 

  • Astington J.W., Jenkins J.M. A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology 1999; 35: 1311–1320.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blake I.K. The social-emotional orientation of mother-child communication in African American Families. International Journal of Behavioural Development 1993; 16: 443–463.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blake, I.K. “Language development and socialization in young African-American children.” In Cross-cultural roots of minority child development, P.M. Greenfield, R.R. Cocking (Eds.), Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L. The Transition from Infancy to Language: Acquiring the Power of Expression. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press, 1993.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brice Heath, S. Ways with words. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press, 1983.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brice Heath S. Oral and literate traditions among Black Americans living in poverty American Psychologist 1989; 44: 367–373.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutting A.L., Dunn J. Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language, and family background: Individual differences and interrelations. Child Development 1999; 70:853–865.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Villiers, J. “Language and theory of mind: What are the developmental relationships.” In Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (2nd Ed.), S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, D. Cohen (Eds.), Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Villiers, J.G., de Villiers, P.A. “Linguistic determinism and the understanding of false beliefs”. In Children’s reasoning and the mind, P. Mitchell, K. Riggs (Eds.), Hove, Sussex: Psychology Press, 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Villiers, J., Pyers, J. Complementing cognition: The relationship between language and theory of mind. In Proceedings of the 21st Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  • Delgado-Gaitan, C. “Socializing young children in Mexican-American families: An intergenerational perspective.” In Cross-cultural roots of minority child development, P.M. Greenfield, R.R. Cocking (Eds.) Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn J., Brown J., Slomkowski C., Tesla C., Youngblade L. Young children’s understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents. Child Development 1991; 62: 1352–1366.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell J.H., Flavell E.R., Green F.L. Development of the appearance-reality distinction. Cognitive Psychology 1983; 15: 95–120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell J.H., Green F.L., Flavell E.R. Development of knowledge about the appearance-reality distinction. Monographs of the Society for research in Child Development 1986; 51, 1: Serial No. 212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fu-Xi, F., Keats, D.M. ““The Master and the Wolf”: A study in the development of social perspective taking in Chinese and Australian children.” In Heterogeneity in crosscultural psychology, D.M. Keats, D. Munro, L. Mann (Eds.), Amsterdam: Swets, Zeitlinger B.V, 1989.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geertz C. On the nature of anthropological understanding. American Scientist 1975; 63: 47–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, R., Lucariello, J. “Role of learning in cognitive development.” In Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology: Vol. 3. Learning, motivation, and emotion (3rd ed.), H. Pashler (Series Ed.), C.R. Gallistel (Vol. Ed.), New York: Wiley, 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  • German, T., Leslie, A. “Attending to and learning about mental states.” In Children’s reasoning and the mind, P. Mitchell, K. Riggs (Eds.), Hove, U.K.: Psychology Press, 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  • German T.P., Leslie A.M. Children’s inferences from knowing to pretending and believing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 2001; 19: 59–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg, C., Gallimore, R. “Immigrant Latino parents’ values and beliefs about their children’s education: Continuities and discontinuities across cultures and generations.” In Advances in motivation and achievement: Culture, motivation and achievement (Vol. 9), M. L. Maehr, P.R. Pintrich (Eds.), Connecticut: Jai Press, 1995.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik A. How we know our minds: The illusions of first person knowledge of intentionality. Behavioural and Brain Sciences 1993; 16: 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik A., Astington J.W. Children’s understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction. Child Development 1988; 59: 26–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik A., Slaughter V. Young children’s understanding of changes in their mental states. Child Development 1991; 62: 98–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., Wellman, H.M. “The theory theory.” In Domain specificity in cognition and culture, L. Hirschfeld, S. Gelman (Eds.), New York: Cambridge University Press, 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield, P.M. “Independence and interdependence as developmental scripts: Implications for theory, research, and practice.” In Cross-cultural roots of minority child development, P.M. Greenfield, R.R. Cocking (Eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield P.M., Bruner J.S. Culture and cognitive growth. International Journal of Psychology 1966; 1: 89–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner D., Harris P.L., Ohmoto M., Hamazaki T. Japanese children’s understanding of the distinction between real and apparent emotion. International Journal of Behaviour and Development 1988; 11: 203–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gross D., Harris P.L. False beliefs about emotion: Children’s understanding of misleading emotional displays. International Journal of Behaviour and Development 1988; 11: 475–488.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hale C.M., Tager-Flusberg H. The influence of language on theory of mind: A training study. Developmental Science 2003; 6: 346–359.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M.A.K., Learning how to mean: Explorations in the development of language. London: Edward Arnold, 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  • Happe’ F.G.E. The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development 1995; 66: 843–855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris P.L. From simulation to folk psychology: The case for development. Mind and Language 1992; 7: 120–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris P.L., Donnelly K., Guz G.R., Pitt-Watson R. Children’s understanding of the distinction between real and apparent emotion. Child Development 1986; 57: 895–909.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harris P.L., Johnson C.N., Hutton D., Andrews G., Cooke T. Young children’s theory of mind and emotion, Cognition and Emotion 1989; 3: 379–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hresko, W.P., Reid, D.K., Hammill, D., Test of Early Language Development (3rd Edition). Austin, Texas: Pro-ed, 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes C., Dunn J. Understanding mind and emotion: Longitudinal associations with mentalstate talk between young friends. Developmental Psychology 1998; 34: 1026–1037.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins J.M., Astington J.W. Cognitive factors and family structure associated with theory of mind development in young children. Developmental Psychology 1996; 32: 70–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie A.M., Polizzi P. Inhibitory processing in the false belief task: Two conjectures. Developmental Science 1998; 1: 247–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie A.M., Thaiss L. Domain specificity in conceptual development: Neuropsychological evidence from autism. Cognition 1992; 43: 225–251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis C., Freeman N.H., Kyriakidou C., Maridaki-Kassotaki K., Berridge D.M. Social influences on false belief access: Specific sibling influences or general apprenticeship? Child Development 1996; 67: 2930–2947.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lohmann H., Tomasello M. The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: A training study. Child Development 2003; 74: 1130–1144.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lucariello, J. “New insights into the functions, development, and origins of theory of mind: the Functional Multilinear Socialization (FMS) Model. In The development of the mediated mind: Sociocultural context and cognitive development, J.M. Lucariello, J.A. Hudson, R. Fivush, P.J. Bauer (Eds.), Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lucariello J., Durand T., Tatelman T., Yarnell L. Low-income children’s cognitive capital: Strength in social critical thinking. In Understanding human development to inform educational practice. Paper session conducted at the American Education Research Association, San Diego, 2004 April.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lucariello, J., Durand, T., Yarnell, L., Evidence for a differentiated theory of mind in kindergarten children. Manuscript in preparation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markus H.R., Kitayama S. Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review 1991; 98: 224–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mead, G.H., Mind, self & society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1934.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P., Wendy, Amy, Beth: Learning language in South Baltimore. Austin: University of Texas Press, 1982.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. “Teasing as language socialization and verbal play in a white working-class community.” In Language socialization across cultures. Studies in the social and cultural foundations of language, No. 3, B.B. Schieffelin, E. Ochs (Eds.), New York: Cambridge University Press, 1986.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller P. The socialization of anger and aggression. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 1987; 33: 1–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller P. Early talk about the past: The origins of conversational stories of personal experience. Journal of Child Language 1988; 15: 293–315.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore C., Barresi J., Thompson C. The cognitive basis of future-oriented prosocial behaviour. Social Development 1998; 7: 198–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson K. Individual differences in language development: Implications for development and language. Developmental Psychology 1981; 17: 170–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D.R. “On the origins of beliefs and other intentional states in children.” In Developing theories of mind, J.W. Astington, P. Harris, D.R. Olson, (Eds.), Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press, 1988.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J., Understanding the Representational Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1991.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perner J., Ruffman T., Leekam S.R. Theory of mind is contagious: You catch it from your sibs. Child Development 1994; 65: 1228–1238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson C.C., Siegal M. Representing inner worlds: Theory of mind in autistic, deaf, and normal hearing children. Psychological Science 1999; 10: 126–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruffman T., Perner J., Naito M., Parkin L., Clements W.A. Older (but not younger) siblings facilitate false belief understanding. Developmental Psychology 1998; 34: 161–174.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruffman T., Slade L., Crowe E. The relation between children’s and mothers’ mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development 2002; 73: 734–751.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scholl B.J., Leslie A.M. Modularity, development, and ‘theory of mind.’ Mind And Language 1999; 14: 131–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scholl B.J., Leslie A.M. Minds, modules, and meta-analysis. Child Development 2001; 72:696–701.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegal, M., Knowing children: Experiments in conversation and cognition. Hove, England: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis, 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegal M. Language and thought: The fundamental significance of conversational awareness for cognitive development. Developmental Science 1999; 2: 1–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegal, M., Peterson, C.C. “Children’s theory of mind and the conversational territory of cognitive development.” In Origins of an understanding of mind, C. Lewis, P. Mitchell (Eds.), Hove, England: Erlbaum, 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegal M., Peterson C.C. Breaking the mold: A fresh look at questions about children’s understanding of lies and mistakes. Developmental Psychology 1996; 32: 322–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegal M., Varley R., Want S.C. Mind over grammar: Reasoning in aphasia and development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2001; 5: 296–301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C.E. “Conversations with children.” In Language acquisition, P. Fletcher, M. Garman (Eds.), 1986.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow C.E. The theoretical basis for relationships between language and literacy in development. Journal of Research in Childhood Education 1991; 6: 5–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vinden P.G. Junin Quechua children’s understanding of mind. Child Development 1996; 67:1707–1716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vinden, P.G., Astington, J.W. “Culture and understanding other minds.” In Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience, S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, D.J. Cohen (Eds.), Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman H.M., Cross D. Theory of mind and conceptual change: Reply. Child Development 2001; 72: 702–707.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman H.M., Cross D., Watson J. Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development 2001; 72: 655–684.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woolfe T., Want S.C., Siegal M. Signposts to development: Theory of mind in deaf children. Child Development 2002; 73: 768–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2006 Springer Science+Business Media, Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Lucariello, J., Le Donne, M., Durand, T., Yarnell, L. (2006). Social and Intrapersonal Theories of Mind. In: Antonietti, A., Liverta-Sempio, O., Marchetti, A. (eds) Theory of Mind and Language in Developmental Contexts. The Springer Series on Human Exceptionality. Springer, Boston, MA . https://doi.org/10.1007/0-387-24997-4_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics