Skip to main content

Mind Over Grammar

Reasoning in Aphasia and Development

  • Chapter
Theory of Mind and Language in Developmental Contexts

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Akhtar N. et al. The role of discourse novelty in children’s early word learning. Child Development 1996; 67: 637–645

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Astington J.W., Jenkins J.M. A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory of-mind development. Developmental Psychology 1999; 35: 1311–1320

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin D. Early referential understanding: Infants’ ability to recognize referential acts for what they are. Developmental Psychology 1993; 29: 832–843

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen S., Leslie A.M., Frith U. Does the autistic child have theory of mind? Cognition 1985; 21: 37–46

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bickerton, D., Language and Human Behaviour. Seattle: University of Washington Press, 1995.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop D.V.M. Development of the children’s communication checklist (CCC): A method for assessing qualitative aspects of communicative impairment in children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 1998; 39: 879–891

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom P. Language and thought: Does grammar make us smart? Current Biology 2000; 10:R516–517

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom P, Markson L. Capacities underlying word learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 1999; 3: 286–288

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowerman, M, Levinson, S.C., Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown J.R. et al. Why talk about mental states? The significance of children’s conversations with friends, siblings and mothers. Child Development 1996; 67: 836–849

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown R. Linguistic determinism and the part of speech. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 1957; 55: 1–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S., “Whorf versus continuity theorists: Bringing data to bear on the debate.” In Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development, M. Bowerman, S.C. Levinson (Eds.), Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey S., Spelke E.S. Science and core knowledge. Philosophy of Science 1996; 63: 515–533.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter M. et al. Fourteen-through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behaviour and Development 1998; 21: 315–330

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers, P. Language, thought and consciousness: An essay in philosophical psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 1998.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N., Rules and representations. Oxford, UK: Blackwell, 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark E.V. Conceptual perspective and lexical choice in acquisition. Cognition 1997; 64: 1–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H.H, Brennan, S.E. “Grounding in communication.” In Perspectives on socially shared cognition, L.B. Resnick, J.M. Levine, S.D. Teasley (Eds.), Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 1991.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csbira G. et al. Goal attribution without agency cues: The perception of ‘pure reason’ in infancy. Cognition, 1999; 72: 237–267

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Custer W.L. A comparison of young children’s understanding of contradictory representations in pretense, memory, and belief. Child Development 1996; 67: 678–688

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Villiers J.G., Pyers J.E. Complements to cognition: A longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding. Cognitive Development 2002;17: 1037–1060.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deák G.O, Maratsos M. On having a complex representation of things: Preschoolers use multiple words for objects and people. Developmental Psychology 1998; 34: 224–240

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar R.I.M. Co-evolution of neocortex size, group size and language in humans. Behavioural and Brain Sciences 1993; 16: 681–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fritzley V.H., Lee K. Do young children always say yes to yes-no questions? A metadevelopmental study of the affirmation bias. Child Development 2003; 74: 1297–1313.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garrett, M. “Production of speech: observations from normal and pathological language use.” In Normality and pathology in cognitive functions, A. Ellis (Ed.), London: Academic Press, 1982.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman R. First principles organize attention to and learning about relevant data — number and the animate-inanimate distinction. Cognitive Science 1990; 14: 79–106

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman S.A., Bloom P. Young children are sensitive to how an object was created when deciding how to name it. Cognition 2000; 76: 91–103

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H. P. “Logic and conversation”. In Syntax and semantics, Vol. 3: Speech acts, P. Cole, J.L. Morgan (Eds.), Academic Press, 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  • Happé F. et al. Acquired ‘theory of mind’ impairments following stroke. Cognition 1999; 70: 211–240

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heaton, R.K. et al., Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Psychological Assessment Resource, 1993.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herman, R. et al., Assessing BSL Development: Receptive Skills Test. London: Forest Books, 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilton D.J. The social context of reasoning: Conversational inference and rational judgment. Psychological Bulletin 1995; 118: 248–271

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hollich G.J. et al. Breaking the language barrier: An emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 2000; 65: Serial No. 262

    Google Scholar 

  • Kertesz, A., “Cognitive function in severe aphasia.” In Thought without language, L. Weiskrantz (Ed.), Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 1988.

    Google Scholar 

  • Legerstee M. et al. Precursors to the development of intention at 6 months: Understanding people and their actions. Developmental Psychology 2000; 36: 627–637

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie A.M., Frith U. Autistic children’s understanding of seeing, knowing and believing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 1988; 6: 315–324

    Google Scholar 

  • Macnamara, J., Names for Things, MIT Press, 1982.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler J.M., McDonough L. Drinking and driving don’t mix: Inductive generalization in infancy. Cognition 1996; 59: 307–335

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E.M., “The acquisition and hierarchical organization of categories by children.” In Origins of cognitive skills, C. Sophian (Ed.), N.J. Hillsdale; Erlbaum, 1984.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marschark M. et al. Understanding theory of mind in children who are deaf. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2000; 41: 1067–1073

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin R. STM and sentence processing: Evidence from neuropsychology. Memory and Cognition 1993; 21: 176–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meadow, K.P. “The development of deaf children”. In Review of Child Development Research, Vol. 5, E.M. Hetherington (Ed.), Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff A.N. Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology 1995; 31: 838–850

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merriman W. et al. How shall a thing be called? Journal of Child Language 1995; 22: 129–149

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nowak M.A., Krakauer, D.C. The evolution of language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 2000; 96: 8028–8033

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perner J. et al. Three-year-olds’ difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 1987; 5: 125–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson C.C. Theory of mind in children with cochlear implants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, in press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson C.C, Siegal M. Insights into theory of mind from deafness and autism. Mind and Language 2000; 15: 123–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C.C, Siegal, M., “Psychological, physical, and biological thinking in normal, autistic, and deaf children.” In The emergence of core domains of thought: New directions for child development, No. 75, H.M. Wellman, K. Inagaki (Eds.), San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson C.C, Siegal M. Changing focus on the representational mind: Deaf, autistic and normal children’s concepts of false photos, false drawings, and false beliefs. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 1998; 16: 301–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson C.C., Siegal M. Representing inner worlds: Theory of mind in autistic, deaf, and normal hearing children. Psychological Science 1999; 10: 126–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell P.A. et al. The development of theory of mind in deaf children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 1998; 39: 903–910

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sapp F. et al. Three-year-olds’ difficulty with the appearance-reality distinction: Is it real or is it apparent? Developmental Psychology 2000; 36: 547–560.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe R., Carey S., Kanwisher N. Understanding other minds: Linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. Annual Review of Psychology 2004; 55: 87–124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schafer G., Plunkett K. Rapid word learning by fifteen-month-olds under tightly controlled conditions. Child Development 1998; 69: 309–320

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scholl B.J., Tremoulet P.D. Perceptual causality and animacy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2000; 4: 299–309

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Segal, G. “Representing representations”, in Language and thought: Interdisciplinary themes, P. Carruthers & J (Eds.), Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 1996.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegal, M., Knowing children: Experiments in conversation and cognition (2nd Ed.). Hove, UK: Psychology Press, 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegal M. Beyond methodology: Frequently asked questions on the significance of conversation for development. Developmental Science 1999; 2: 29–34

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegal, M., Surian, L. Modularity in language and theory of mind: What is the evidence? Paper presented at the Conference on Innateness and the Structure of the Mind; Sheffield, UK; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegal M., Varley R. Neural systems underlying theory of mind. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2002; 3: 463–471

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith M., Apperly M., White V. False belief reasoning and the acquisition of relative clauses. Child Development 2003; 74: 1709–1719.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Surian L., Siegal M. Sources of performance on theory of mind tasks in right hemisphere damaged patients. Brain and Language 2001; 78: 224–232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg H., Sullivan K. A componential view of theory of mind: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognition 2000; 76: 59–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tye-Murray N. Conversational fluency of children who use cochlear implants. Ear and Hearing 2003; 24: 82S–89S.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Lely H.K.J. et al. Evidence for a grammar-specific deficit in children. Current Biol. 1998; 8: 1253–1258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Varley, R. “Science without grammar: scientific reasoning in severe agrammatic aphasia”. In Cognitive Bases of Science, P. Carruthers, S. Stich, M. Siegal (Eds.), Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varley R., Siegal M. Evidence for cognition without grammar from causal reasoning and ‘theory of mind’ in an agrammatic aphasic patient. Current Biol. 2000; 10: 723–726

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Varley R., Siegal M., Want S. Severe impairment in grammar does not preclude theory of mind. Neurocase 2001; 7: 489–493.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer H., Perner J. Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition 1983; 13: 103–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woodward A.L. Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition 1999; 69: 1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woolfe T., Want S.C., Siegal M. Signposts to development: Theory of mind in deaf children. Child Development 2002; 73: 768–778.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2006 Springer Science+Business Media, Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Siegal, M., Varley, R., Want, S.C. (2006). Mind Over Grammar. In: Antonietti, A., Liverta-Sempio, O., Marchetti, A. (eds) Theory of Mind and Language in Developmental Contexts. The Springer Series on Human Exceptionality. Springer, Boston, MA . https://doi.org/10.1007/0-387-24997-4_6

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics