Lost Classics and Forgotten Contributors

James Mark Baldwin as a Case Study in the Disappearance and Rediscovery of Ideas
  • Robert H. Wozniak
Part of the Path in Psychology book series

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Angell, J. R. (1907). The province of functional psychology. The Psychological Review, 14, 61–91.Google Scholar
  2. Ash, M. (1995). Gestalt psychology in German culture, 1890–1967: holism and the quest for objectivity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  3. Bain, A. (1855). The senses and the intellect. London: Parker.Google Scholar
  4. Bain, A. (1859). The emotions and the will. London: Parker.Google Scholar
  5. Baldwin, J. M. (1890). Origin of right or left handedness. Science, 16, 247–248.Google Scholar
  6. Baldwin, J. M. (1891a). Handbook of psychology: feeling and will. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
  7. Baldwin, J. M. (1891b). Suggestion in infancy. Science, 17, 113–117.Google Scholar
  8. Baldwin, J. M. (1892a). Suggestion and will. In International congress of experimental psychology, second session-London, 1892 (pp. 49–56). London: Williams & Norgate.Google Scholar
  9. Baldwin, J. M. (1892b). Infants’ movements. Science, 19, 15–16.Google Scholar
  10. Baldwin, J. M. (1893). Distance and color perception by infants. Science, 21, 231–232.Google Scholar
  11. Baldwin, J. M. (1895). Mental development in the child and the race; methods and processes. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  12. Baldwin, J. M. (1896). A new factor in evolution. The American Naturalist, 30, 441–451, 536–553.Google Scholar
  13. Baldwin, J. M. (1897). Social and ethical interpretations in mental development; a study in social psychology. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  14. Baldwin, J. M. (1898). On selective thinking. The Psychological Review, 5, 1–24.Google Scholar
  15. Baldwin, J. M. (Ed.) (1901–1905). Dictionary of philosophy and psychology. 3 vols. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  16. Baldwin, J. M. (1902). Development and evolution, including psychophysical evolution, evolution by orthoplasy, and the theory of genetic modes. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  17. Baldwin, J. M. (1906–1911). Thought and things. A study of the development and meaning of thought or genetic logic. 3 vols. London: Swan Sonnenschein.Google Scholar
  18. Baldwin, J. M. (1913). History of psychology: a sketch and an interpretation. 2 vols. London: Watts.Google Scholar
  19. Baldwin, J. M. (1926). Between two wars, 1861–1921; being memories, opinions and letters received. 2 vols. Boston: Stratford.Google Scholar
  20. Baldwin, J. M. (1930). James Mark Baldwin. In C. Murchison (Ed.), A history of psychology in autobiography (Vol. 1, pp. 1–30). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.Google Scholar
  21. Baldwin, J. M. & Shaw, W. J. (1895a). Memory for square-size. Studies from the Princeton Laboratory I. The Psychological Review, 2, 236–239.Google Scholar
  22. Baldwin, J. M. & Shaw, W. J. (1895b). Types of reaction. Studies fromthe Princeton Laboratory IV. The Psychological Review, 2, 259–273.Google Scholar
  23. Bernheim, H. (1886). De la suggestion et de ses applications à la thérapeutique. Paris: Doin.Google Scholar
  24. Binet, A. (1890a). La perception des longeurs et des nombres chez quelques petits enfants. Revue philosophique, 30, 68–81.Google Scholar
  25. Binet, A. (1890b). Perceptions d’enfant. Revue philosophique, 30, 582–611.Google Scholar
  26. Binet, A. (1908). Le développement de l’intelligence chez les enfants. L’Ann’ee psychologique, 14, 1–94.Google Scholar
  27. Binet, A. (1911). Nouvelles recherches sur la mesure du niveau intellectuel chez les enfants d’école. L’Année psychologique, 17, 145–201.Google Scholar
  28. Binet, A. & Henneguy, L. (1894). La psychologie des grands calculateurs et joueurs d’échecs. Paris: Hachette.Google Scholar
  29. Binet, A. & Henri, V. (1894). Recherches sur le d’eveloppement de la mémoire visuelle des enfants. Revue philosophique, 37, 348–350.Google Scholar
  30. Binet, A. & Simon, Th. (1905). Méthodes nouvelles pour le diagnostic du niveau intellectuel des anormaux. LÁnnée psychologique, 11, 191–244.Google Scholar
  31. Blumenthal, A. L. (1975). A reappraisal of Wilhelm Wundt. American Psychologist, 30, 1081–1088.Google Scholar
  32. Blumenthal, A. L. (1980). Wilhelm Wundt-Problems of interpretation. In W. G. Bringmann & R. D. Tweney (Eds.), Wundt studies: a centennial collection (pp. 435–445). Toronto: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
  33. Boring, E. G. (1929). A history of experimental psychology. New York: Century.Google Scholar
  34. Bringmann, W. G., Bringmann, N.J., & Ungerer, G. A. (1980). The establishment of Wundt’s laboratory: an archival and documentary study. In W. G. Bringmann & R. D. Tweney (Eds.), Wundt studies: a centennial collection (pp. 123–157). Toronto: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
  35. Broughton, J. M. & Freeman-Moir, D. J. (Eds.) (1982). The cognitive-developmental psychology of James Mark Baldwin: Current theory and research in genetic epistemology. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  36. Burack, B. (1949). An electrical logic machine. Science, 109, 610.Google Scholar
  37. Burrow, T. (1927). The social basis of consciousness. A study in organic psychology based upon a synthetic and societal concept of the neuroses. London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner.Google Scholar
  38. Burrow, T. (1949). The neurosis of man. An introduction to a science of human behaviour. New York: Harcourt, Brace.Google Scholar
  39. Cairns, R. B. (1980). Developmental theory before Piaget: the remarkable contributions of James Mark Baldwin. Contemporary Psychology, 25, 438–440.Google Scholar
  40. Cairns, R. B. (1983). The emergence of developmental psychology. In P.H. Mussen (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 41–102). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  41. Campbell, D. T. (1982). The “blind-variation-and-selective-retention“ theme. In J. M. Broughton & D. J. Freeman-Moir (Eds.), The cognitive-developmental psychology of James Mark Baldwin (pp. 87–97). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  42. Candolle, A. de (1872). Histoire des sciences et des savants depuis deux siécles. Geneve: Georg.Google Scholar
  43. Carpenter, W. B. (1874). Principles of mental physiology, with their applications to the training and discipline of the mind, and the study of its morbid conditions. London: King.Google Scholar
  44. Chomsky, N. (1959). A review of Skinner’s “Verbal Behavior.” Language, 35, 26–58.Google Scholar
  45. Danziger, K. (1980). Wundt and the two traditions of psychology. In R.W. Rieber (Ed.), Wilhelm Wundt and the making of a scientific psychology (pp. 73–87). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  46. Dewey, J. (1896). The reflex arc concept in psychology. The Psychological Review, 3, 357–370.Google Scholar
  47. Dewey, J. (1903). Studies in logical theory. Chicago: University of Chicago.Google Scholar
  48. Dewey, J. (1916). Democracy and education. An introduction to the philosophy of education. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  49. Dunlap, K. (1932). Knight Dunlap. In C. Murchison (Ed.), A history of psychology in autobiography (Vol. 2, pp. 35–61). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.Google Scholar
  50. Dunn, L. C (1966). The American Naturalist in American biology. American Naturalist, 100, 481–492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Ebbinghaus, H. (1885). Über das gedächtnis. Untersuchungen zur experimentellen psychologie. Leipzig: Duncker & Humblot.Google Scholar
  52. Ehrenfels, C. von (1890). Ueber gestaltqualitäten. Vierteljahrsschrift für wissenschaftliche Philosophie, 14, 249–292.Google Scholar
  53. Evans, R. & Scott, F. J. D. (1978). The 1913 International Congress of Psychology: the American congress that wasn’t. American Psychologist, 33, 711–723.Google Scholar
  54. Fancher, R. E. (1979). Pioneers of psychology. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  55. Fechner, G. Th. (1860). Elemente der psychophysik. Leipzig: Breitkopf und Härtel.Google Scholar
  56. Ferrier, D. (1876). The functions of the brain. London: Smith, Elder.Google Scholar
  57. Galton, F. (1869). Hereditary genius: an inquiry into its laws and consequences. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  58. Galton, F. (1874). English men of science: their nature and nurture. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  59. Galton, F. (1883). Inquiries into human faculty and its development. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  60. Gardner, H. (1985). The mind’s new science: a history of the cognitive revolution. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  61. Gillin, J. L. (1927). Franklin Henry Giddings. In H. W. Odum (Ed.), American masters of social science (pp. 191–228). New York: Holt.Google Scholar
  62. Gould, S. J. (1980). The panda’s thumb: more reflections in natural history. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  63. Greenwood, J. D. (1999). Understanding the “cognitive revolution” in psychology. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 35, 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  65. Helmholtz, H. von (1863). Die lehre von dem tonempfindungen als physiologische grundlage f ür die theorie der musik. Braunschweig: Vieweg.Google Scholar
  66. Helmholtz, H. von (1867). Handbuch der physiologischen optik. Leipzig: Voss.Google Scholar
  67. Herbart, J. F. (1824/1825). Psychologie als wissenschaft, neu gegründet auf erfahrung, metaphysik und mathematik. Königsberg: Unzer.Google Scholar
  68. Hinton, G. F. & Nowlan, S. J. (1987). How learning can guide evolution. Complex Systems, 1, 495–502.Google Scholar
  69. Hobhouse, L. T. (1901). Mind in evolution. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  70. Höffding, H. (1882). Psykologi i omrids paa grundlag af erfaring. Kobenhaven: Philipsens.Google Scholar
  71. Huey, E. B. (1908). The psychology and pedagogy of reading. With a review of the history of reading and writing and of methods, texts, and hygiene in reading. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  72. Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior. An introduction to behavior theory. New York: Appleton-Century.Google Scholar
  73. Jackson, J. H. (1932). Selected writings of John Hughlings Jackson. 2 vols. Edited by J. Taylor. London: Hodder and Stoughton.Google Scholar
  74. James, W. (1884). On some omissions of introspective psychology. Mind, 9, 1–26.Google Scholar
  75. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. 2 vols. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
  76. James, W. (1992–2002). The correspondence of William James. Edited by I.K. Skrupskelis & E.M. Berkeley. 10 vols. Charlottesville, VA: University Press of Virginia.Google Scholar
  77. Janet, P. (1889). L’automatisme psychologique. Paris: Alcan.Google Scholar
  78. Joas, H. (1985). G. H. Mead. A contemporary re-examination of his thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  79. Kant, I. (1786). Metaphysische anfangsgr ünde der naturwissenschaft. Riga: Hartknoch.Google Scholar
  80. Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of gestalt psychology. New York: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
  81. Külpe, O. (1893). Grundriss der psychologie. Auf experimenteller grundlage dargestellt. Leipzig: Engelmann.Google Scholar
  82. Ladd, G. T. (1887). Elements of physiological psychology. A treatise of the activities and nature of the mind from the physical and experimental point of view. New York: Scribner’s.Google Scholar
  83. Lewes, G. H. (1877). The physical basis of mind. With illustrations. Being the second series of problems of life and mind. London: Trübner.Google Scholar
  84. Lotze, R. H. (1881). Grundzüge der psychologie. Dictate aus den vorlesungen. Leipzig: Hirzel.Google Scholar
  85. Mach, E. (1886). Beiträge zur analyse der empfindungen. Jena: Fischer.Google Scholar
  86. Maudsley, H. (1870). Body and mind: an inquiry into their connection and mutual infiuence, specially in reference to mental disorders. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  87. McDougall, W. (1908). An introduction to social psychology. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
  88. Morgan, C. L. (1894). Introduction to comparative psychology. London: Scott.Google Scholar
  89. Morgan, C. L. (1896). Habit and instinct. London: Arnold.Google Scholar
  90. Münsterberg, H. (1909). The eternal values. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  91. Münsterberg, H. (1913). Psychology and industrial efficiency. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  92. Murray, D. (1995). Gestalt psychology and the cognitive revolution. New York: Harvester Wheatsheaf.Google Scholar
  93. Nicolas, S. & Ferrand, L. (2002). Alfred Binet and higher education. History of Psychology, 5, 264–283.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. O’Donnell, J. M. (1979). The crisis of experimentalism in the 1920s: E.G. Boring and his uses of history. American Psychologist, 34, 289–295.Google Scholar
  95. Pfungst, O. (1907). Das pferd des Herrn von Osten (der kluge Hans). Ein beitrag zur experimentellen tier-und menschen-psychologie. Leipzig: Barth.Google Scholar
  96. Piaget, J. (1936). La naissance de l’intelligence chez l’enfant. Neuchatel: Delachaux & Niestlé.Google Scholar
  97. Piaget, J. (1982). Reflections on Baldwin. In J. M. Broughton & D.J. Freeman-Moir (Eds.), The cognitive-developmental psychology of James Mark Baldwin (pp. 80–86). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  98. Pollack, R. H. & Brenner, M. J. (1969). Editors’ introduction. In A. Binet. The experimental psychology of Alfred Binet (pp. ix–xiv). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  99. Prince, M. (1906). The dissociation of a personality. A biographical study in abnormal psychology. New York: Longmans, Green.Google Scholar
  100. Ribot, Th. (1879). La Psychologie allemande contemporaine (École expérimentale). Paris: Baillière.Google Scholar
  101. Ribot, Th. (1886). German psychology of to-day. The empirical school. New York: Scribner’s.Google Scholar
  102. Richards, R. J. (1987). Darwin and the emergence of evolutionary theories of mind and behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  103. Romanes, G. J. (1888). Mental evolution in man. Origin of human faculty. London: Kegan Paul, Trench.Google Scholar
  104. Royce, J. (1895). Self-consciousness and social consciouness. The Philosophical Review, 4, 465–485, 577–602.Google Scholar
  105. Samelson, F. (1981). Struggle for scientific authority: The reception of Watson’s behaviorism, 1913–1920. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 17, 399–425.Google Scholar
  106. Scott, W. D. (1908). The psychology of advertising. A simple exposition of the principles of psychology in their relation to successful advertising. Boston: Small, Maynard.Google Scholar
  107. Shook, J. R. (1995). Wilhelm Wundt’s contribution to John Dewey’s functional psychology. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 31, 347–369.Google Scholar
  108. Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms; an experimental analysis. New York: Century.Google Scholar
  109. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  110. Spencer, H. (1855). The principles of psychology. London: Longman, Brown, Green, and Longmans.Google Scholar
  111. Taine, H. (1870). De l’intelligence. 2 vols. Paris: Hachette.Google Scholar
  112. Thorndike, E. L. (1904). An introduction to the theory of mental and social measurements. New York: The Science Press.Google Scholar
  113. Thorndike, E. L. (1911). Animal intelligence. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  114. Thorndike, E. L. (1913/1914). Educational psychology. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University.Google Scholar
  115. Titchener, E. B. (1896). An outline of psychology. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  116. Tolman, E. C. (1938). Purposive behavior in animals and men. NY: Century.Google Scholar
  117. Tweney, R. D. & Yachanin, S.A. (1980). Titchener’s Wundt. In W. G. Bringmann & R.D. Tweney (Eds.), Wundt studies: a centennial collection (pp. 380–395). Toronto: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
  118. Valsiner, J. & Van der Veer, R. (1988). On the social nature of human cognition: An analysis of the shared intellectual roots of George Herbert Mead and Lev Vygotsky. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 18, 117–136.Google Scholar
  119. Van der Veer, R. & Valsiner, J. (1988). Lev Vygotsky and Pierre Janet: On the origin of the concept of sociogenesis. Developmental Review, 8, 52–65.Google Scholar
  120. Washburn, M. F. (1935). James Mark Baldwin: 1861–1934. The American Journal of Psychology, 47, 168–169.Google Scholar
  121. Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. The Psychological Review, 20, 158–177.Google Scholar
  122. Wozniak, R. H. (1997). Behaviorism. In W. G. Bringmann, H.E. Lück, R. Miller, & C.E. Early (Eds.), A pictorial history of psychology (pp. 198–205). München/Carol Stream, IL: Quintessence.Google Scholar
  123. Wozniak, R. H. (1999). Classics in psychology, 1855–1914. Historical essays. Bristol & Tokyo: Thoemmes Press/Maruzen Co.Google Scholar
  124. Wozniak, R. H. (2001). Development and synthesis: An introduction to the life and works of James Mark Baldwin. In J. M. Baldwin, Mental development in the child and the race: methods and processes (pp. v–xxxi). Bristol, UK: Thoemmes Press.Google Scholar
  125. Wundt, W. (1862). Beiträge zur theorie der sinneswahrnehmung. Leipzig und Heidelberg: Winter.Google Scholar
  126. Wundt, W. (1863). Vorlesungen über die menschen-und thierseele. Leipzig: Voss.Google Scholar
  127. Wundt, W. (1873/1874). Grundzüge der physiologischen psychologie. Leipzig: Engelmann.Google Scholar
  128. Wundt, W. (1896). Grundriss der psychologie. Leipzig: Engelmann.Google Scholar
  129. Wundt, W. & Diamond, S. (1980). Selected texts from writings of Wilhelm Wundt. In R. W. Rieber (Ed.), Wilhelm Wundt and the making of a scientific psychology (pp. 155–177). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  130. Yerkes, R. M. (1903). Development and evolution. The American Naturalist, 37, 347–348.Google Scholar
  131. Young, R. M. (1970). Mind, brain, and adaptation in the nineteenth century. Cerebral localization and its biological context from Gall to Ferrier. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science + Business Media, Inc. 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  • Robert H. Wozniak

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations