Understanding Social Intelligence

  • Per Persson
  • Jarmo Laaksolahti
  • Peter Löonnqvist
Part of the Multiagent Systems, Artificial Societies, and Simulated Organizations book series (MASA, volume 3)

Abstract

Believable social interaction is not only about agents that look right but also do the right thing. To achieve this we must consider the everyday knowledge and expectations by which users make sense of real, fictive or artificial social beings. This folk-theoretical understanding of other social beings involves several, rather independent, levels such as expectations on behaviour, expectations on primitive psychology, models of folk-psychology, understanding of traits, social roles and empathy. Implications for Socially Intelligent A gents (SIA) research are discussed.

Keywords

Folk 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. [1]
    An Experimental Study of Apparent Behavior. F. Heider and M. Simmel. American Journal of Psychology, 57:243–259, 1944.Google Scholar
  2. [2]
    Andrew Whiten. Natural Theories of Mind. Evolution, Development and Simulation of Everyday Mindreading. Basil Blackwell, Oxford, 1991.Google Scholar
  3. [3]
    Aronson. The Social Animal, Fifth Edition. W. H. Freeman, San Francisco, 1988.Google Scholar
  4. [4]
    B. L. Omdahl. Cognitive Appraisal, Emotion, and Empathy. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, New Jersey, 1995.Google Scholar
  5. [5]
    B. Reeves and C. Nass. The Media Equation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England, 1996.Google Scholar
  6. [6]
    C. Pelachaud and N. I. Badler and M. Steedman. Generating Facial Expression for Speech. Cognitive Science, 20:1–46, 1996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. [7]
    Chris Kleinke. Gaze and Eye Contact: A Research Review.Psychological Bulletin, 100:78–100, 1986.Google Scholar
  8. [8]
    D. C. Dennett. The intentional stance. M.I.T. Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1987.Google Scholar
  9. [9]
    Dorothy Holland and Naomi Quinn. Cultural Models in Language and Thought. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England, 1987.Google Scholar
  10. [10]
    G. Johansson. Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14:201–211, 1973.Google Scholar
  11. [11]
    George Butterworth. The Ontogeny and Phylogeny of Joint Visual Attention. In A. Whiten, editor, Natural Theories of Mind, pages 223–232. Basil Blackwell, Oxford, 1991.Google Scholar
  12. [12]
    George Lakoff and Mark Johnson. Philosophy in the Flesh. The Embodied Mind and its Challenge to Western Thought. Basic Books, New York, 2000.Google Scholar
  13. [13]
    I. Roseman and A. Antoniou and P. Jose. Appraisal Determinants of Emotions: Constructing a More Accurate and Comprehensive Theory. Cognition and Emotion, 10:241–277, 1996.Google Scholar
  14. [14]
    J. Cassell and T. Bickmore and M. Billinghurst and L. Campbell and K. Chang and H. Vilhjáalmsson and H. Yan. Embodiment in Conversational Interfaces: Rea. In ACM CHI 99 Conference Proceedings, Pittsburgh, PA, pages 520–527, 1999.Google Scholar
  15. [15]
    J. Laaksolahti and P. Persson and C. Palo. Evaluating Believability in an Interactive Narrative. In Proceedings of The Second International Conference on Intelligent Agent Technology (IAT2001), October 23–26 2001, Maebashi City, Japan. 2001.Google Scholar
  16. [16]
    J. Perner and S. R. Leekham and H. Wimmer. Three-year-olds’ difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5:125–137, 1987.Google Scholar
  17. [17]
    J. W. Astington. The child’s discovery of the mind. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1993.Google Scholar
  18. [18]
    K. Bartsch and H. M. Wellman. Children talk about the mind. Oxford University Press, Oxford, 1995.Google Scholar
  19. [19]
    K. Dautenhahn. Socially Intelligent Agents and The Primate Social Brain — Towards a Science of Social Minds. In AAAI Fall symposium, Socially Intelligent Agents — The Human in the Loop, North Falmouth, Massachusetts, pages 35–51,2000.Google Scholar
  20. [20]
    Katherine Isbister and Clifford Nass. Consistency of personality in interactive characters: verbal cues, non-verbal cues, and user characteristics. International Journal of Human Computer Studies, pages 251–267, 2000.Google Scholar
  21. [21]
    M. Augoustinos and I. Walker. Social cognition: an integrated introduction. Sage, London, 1995.Google Scholar
  22. [22]
    M. Mateas and A. Stern. Towards Integrating Plot and Character for Interactive Drama. In AAAI Fall Symposium, Socially Intelligent Agents — The Human in the Loop, North Falmouth, MA, pages 113–118, 2000.Google Scholar
  23. [23]
    N. Cantor and W. Mischel. Prototypes in Person Perception. In L. Berkowitz, editor, Advances in Experimental Psychology, volume 12. Academic Press, New York, 1979.Google Scholar
  24. [24]
    P. Ekman. The argument and evidence about universals in facial expressions of emotion. In Handbook of Social Psychophysiology. John Wiley, Chichester, New York, 1989.Google Scholar
  25. [25]
    P. Persson. Understanding Cinema: Constructivism and Spectator Psychology. PhD thesis, Department of Cinema Studies, Stockholm University, 2000. (at http://www.sics.se/perp/.
  26. [26]
    P. Persson and J. Laaksolahti and P. Lonnquist. Understanding Socially Intelligent Agents — A multi-layered phenomenon, 2001. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics, Part A: Systems and Humans, special issue on “Socially Intelligent Agents — The Human in the Loop”,forthcoming.Google Scholar
  27. [27]
    Paola Rizzo. Why Should Agents be Emotional for Entertaining Users? ACrtical Analysis. In Paiva, editor, Affective Interactions. Towards a New Generation of Computer Interfaces, pages 161–181. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, 2000.Google Scholar
  28. [28]
    Paul Harris. Understanding Emotion. In Michael Lewis and Jaenette Haviland, editor, Handbook of Emotions, pages 237–246. The Guilford Press, New York, 1993.Google Scholar
  29. [29]
    Roy D’Andrade. A folk model of the mind. In Dorothy Holland and Naomi Quinn, editor, Cultural Models in Language and Thought, pages 112–148. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England, 1987.Google Scholar
  30. [30]
    S. Marsella. Pedagogical Soap. In AAAI Fall Symposium, Socially Intelligent Agents — The Human in the Loop, North Falmouth, MA, pages 107–112, 2000.Google Scholar
  31. [31]
    S. Planalp and V. DeFrancisco and D. Rutherford. Varieties of Cues to Emotion Naturally Occurring Situations. Cognition and Emotion, 10:137–153, 1996.Google Scholar
  32. [32]
    S. Taylor and J. Crocker. Schematic Bases of Social Information Processes. In T. Higgins and P. Herman and M. Zanna, editor, Social Cognition, pages 89–134. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, New Jersey, 1981.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Kluwer Academic Publishers 2002

Authors and Affiliations

  • Per Persson
    • 1
  • Jarmo Laaksolahti
    • 1
  • Peter Löonnqvist
    • 2
  1. 1.Swedish Institute of Computer ScienceKistaSweden
  2. 2.Department of Computer and Systems SciencesStockholm University and Royal Institute of TechnologyStockholmSweden

Personalised recommendations