Advertisement

Ratchets and Replacement: The Potential Role of Cultural Accumulation in the Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans

  • Michael ChazanEmail author
Chapter
Part of the Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans Series book series (RNMH)

Abstract

The replacement of Neanderthals by modern humans is often explained in terms of an adaptive advantage held by modern humans that is related to cognition. This article explores the possibility that the adaptive advantage of modern humans was related to accumulated cultural knowledge rather than genetically controlled cognitive traits. Using a fictional account of an experiment with social learning robots, the argument is made that differences in meta-population size and differences in the timing of the onset of technological innovations might have led to a disparity between Neanderthals and modern humans in accumulated cultural knowledge. This disparity offers a plausible explanatory framework for the adaptive advantage of modern humans during the expansion into Eurasia.

Keywords

Modern humans Neanderthals Cultural transmission Ratchet effect 

Notes

Acknowledgments

I am very grateful to the organizers of the 2014 RMNH meeting in Hokkaido for inviting me to participate in the conference. This paper builds on the efforts to combine modeling, cognitive science, and archaeology that distinguishes the RMNH framework. I would also like to thank Mark Collard and participants in the HESP program for giving me a chance to discuss these ideas in another stimulating interdisciplinary context.

References

  1. Adler DS, Wilkinson KN, Blockley S, Mark DF, Pinhasi R, Schmidt-Magee BA, Nahapetyan S, Mallol C, Berna F, Glauberman PJ, Raczynski-Henk Y, Wales N, Frahm E, Jöris O, MacLeod A, Smith VC, Cullen VL, Gasparian B (2014) Early Levallois technology and the lower to middle Paleolithic transition in the Southern Caucasus. Science 345(6204):1609–1613CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Aoki K (2015) Modeling abrupt cultural regime shifts during the Palaeolithic and Stone Age. Theory Popul Biol 100:6–12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Aoki K, Feldman MW (2014) Evolution of learning strategies in temporally and spatially variable environments: a review of theory. Theor Popul Biol 91:3–19CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Boëda E (2013) Techno-logique & Technologie: Une Paléohistoire des objets lithiques tranchants. Archéo-Editions, BergeracGoogle Scholar
  5. Bromham L, Hua X, Fitzpatrick TG, Greenhill SJ (2015) Rate of language evolution is affected by population size. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 112(7):2097–2102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Clark A (2008) Supersizing the mind: embodiment, action, and cognitive extension: embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Collard M, Buchanan B, O’Brien MJ (2013a) Population size as an explanation for patterns in the Paleolithic archaeological record. Curr Anthropol 54(S8):S388–S396CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Collard M, Ruttle A, Buchanan B, O’Brien MJ (2013b) Population size and cultural evolution in nonindustrial food-producing societies. PLoS One 8(9):e72628CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cowling SA, Cox PM, Jones CD, Maslin MA, Peros M, Spall S (2008) Simulated glacial and interglacial vegetation across Africa: implications for species phylogenies and trans-African migration of plants and animals. Glob Chang Biol 14:827–840CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dennell RW, Martinón-Torres M, de Castro JMB (2011) Hominin variability, climatic instability and population demography in Middle Pleistocene Europe. Quat Sci Rev 30(11):1511–1524CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hanski I (1998) Metapopulation dynamics. Nature 396:41–49CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Heinrich J (2004) Demography and cultural evolution: why adaptive cultural processes produced maladaptive losses in Tasmania. Am Antiq 69:197–214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Henshilwood CS, Marean CW (2003) The origin of modern human behavior. Curr Anthropol 44:627–651CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hublin JJ, Roebroeks W (2009) Ebb and flow or regional extinctions? On the character of Neandertal occupation of northern environments. Comptes Rendus Palevol 8(5):503–509CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Johnson CR, McBrearty S (2010) 500,000 year old blades from the Kapthurin formation, Kenya. J Hum Evol 58:193–200CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kempe M, Mesoudi A (2014) An experimental demonstration of the effect of group size on cultural accumulation. Evol Hum Behav 35(4):285–290CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Klein RG (2000) Archeology and the evolution of human behavior. Evol Anthropol Issues News Rev 9(1):17–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Kobayashi Y, Aoki K (2012) Innovativeness, population size and cumulative cultural evolution. Theor Popul Biol 82(1):38–47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Mellars P (2006) Why did modern human populations disperse from Africa ca. 60,000 years ago? A new model. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103:9381–9386CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Powell A, Shennan S, Thomas MG (2009) Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science 324(5932):1298–1301CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Premo LS, Kuhn SL (2010) Modeling effects of local extinctions on culture change and diversity in the Paleolithic. PLoS One 5:e15582CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Roebroeks W, Kolen J, Loecker DD (1987) An early Weichselian site at Maastricht-Belvédère (site J), Analecta praehistorica Leidensia Volume 20. University of Leiden, Department of Archaeology, LeidenGoogle Scholar
  23. Simondon G (1958) Du Mode d’Existence des Objets Techniques. Aubier, ParisGoogle Scholar
  24. Tennie C, Call J, Tomasello M (2009) Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Philos Trans R Soc B 364(1528):2405–2415CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Tomasello M (2009) The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MACrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Wilkins J, Chazan M (2012) Blade production∼ 500 thousand years ago at Kathu Pan 1, South Africa: support for a multiple origins hypothesis for early Middle Pleistocene blade technologies. J Archaeol Sci 39:1883–1900CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd. 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of AnthropologyUniversity of TorontoTorontoCanada
  2. 2.Evolutionary Studies InstituteUniversity of the Witwatersrand, WITSJohannesburgSouth Africa

Personalised recommendations