Advertisement

Supporting Heritage Language Development Through Adults’ Participation in Activity Settings

  • Liang LiEmail author
Chapter
Part of the Perspectives in Cultural-Historical Research book series (PCHR, volume 6)

Abstract

The field of heritage language studies is under-researched and bilingual families struggle to maintain their heritage languages at home. The in-depth study reported in this chapter examined an episode of one child, a 4-year-old girl, and her parents, baking at home in order to explore how parents’ participation aligns with children’s actions in supporting their heritage language development in a home activity setting. Hedegaard’s (Studying children: A cultural-historical approach. Open University Press, Maidenhead, pp. 30–45, 2008, Mind, Culture and Activity 16:64–82, 2009) wholeness approach and model of children’s learning and development through participation in institutional practices form the foundation of the analysis. Through taking different perspectives, this chapter explains how the parents interact with their daughter differently while they both aim to support her bilingual heritage learning and development and how this impacts on their daughters’ opportunities to practice Chinese in the home activity setting. This study gives insights into the essence of the activity setting as an analytical concept to research the child’s development.

Keywords

Activity setting Demands Motives Heritage language development Adults’ participation 

Notes

Acknowledgements

I would like to show my gratitude to Professor Marilyn Fleer, who provided invaluable support as supervisor of my doctoral research from which this chapter was developed. I would like to thank all the editors for their comments and contribution to this chapter. I also acknowledge my three researched families’ participation and thank them for welcoming me to video-observe their everyday activities. Feedback on earlier versions of the chapter by colleagues (Dr. Avis Ridgway and Dr. Feiyan Chan) was very much appreciated.

References

  1. Australian Curriculum, Assessment and Reporting Authority (ACARA). (2014). Australian Curriculum. Retrieved from (https://www.australiancurriculum.edu.au/about-the-australian-curriculum/#).Google Scholar
  2. Australian Government. (2012). Australia in the Asian Century White Paper, edited by Australian Government. Canberra: Commonwealth of Australia.Google Scholar
  3. Chaiklin, S. (2012). A conceptual perspective for investigating motive in cultural-historical theory. In M. Hedegaard, A. Edwards, & M. Fleer (Eds.), Motvies in Children’s development (pp. 209–224). New York: Cambridge University.Google Scholar
  4. Cheung, H., Chung, K. K. H., Wong, S. W. L., McBride-Chang, C., Penney, T. B., & Ho, C. S.-H. (2010). Speech perception, metalinguistic awareness, reading, and vocabulary in Chinese-English bilingual children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(2), 367–380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Cummins, J. (2005). A proposal for action: Strategies for recognizing heritage language competence as a learning resource within the mainstream classroom. The Modern Language Journal, 89(4), 585–592.Google Scholar
  6. Esch-Harding, E., & Riley, P. (2003). The bilingual family: A handbook for parents (2nd ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Fairclough, M. A., & Beaudrie, S. M. (2016). Innovative strategies for heritage language teaching: A practical guide for the classroom. (eds). Washington, D.C.: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Fees, B. S., Hoover, L., & Zheng, F. (2014). Chinese kindergarten teachers’ perceived changes in their teaching philosophies and practices: A case study in a university-affiliated program. International Journal of Early Childhood., 46, 231–252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Fleer, M. (2012). The development of motives in children’s play. In M. Hedegaard, A. Edwards, & M. Fleer (Eds.), Motives, emotions and values in children’s and youth’s development: Advances from a cultural-historical approach (pp. 79–96). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Fleer, M. (2014). The demands and motives afforded through digital play in early childhood activtiy settings. Learning, Culture and Socail Interaction., 3(3), 202–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hedegaard, M. (2008). Developing a dialectic approach to researching children’s development. In M. Hedegaard, M. Fleer, J. Bang, & P. Hviid (Eds.), Studying children: A cultural-historical approach (pp. 30–45). Maidenhead, England; New York: Open University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Hedegaard, M. (2009). Children’s development from a cultural-historical approach: Children’s activity in everyday local settings as foundation for their development. Mind, Culture and Activity, 16(1), 64–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Hedegaard, M. (2012). Analyzing children’s learning and development in everyday settings from a cultural-historical wholeness appraoch. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 19(2), 127–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hedegaard, M., & Fleer, M. (2013). Play, learning and children’s development: Everyday life in families and transition to school. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Hedegaard, M., & Chaiklin, S. (2005). Radical–local teaching and learning: A cultural-historical approach. Aarhus: Aarhus University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Hu, J., Torr, J., & Whiteman, P. (2014). Australian Chinese parents’ language attitudes and practices relating to their children’s bilingual development prior to school. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 12(2), 139–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Laurent, A., & Martinot, C. (2009). Bilingualism and phonological segmentation of speech: The case of English-French pre-schoolers. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 9(1), 20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Li, L. (2014). A visual dialectical methodology: using a cultural-historical analysis to unearth the family strategies in children’s bilingual heritage language development. In M. Fleer & A. Ridgway (Eds.), Visual methodologies and digital tools for researching with young children: Transforming visuality (pp. 35–53). Dordrecht: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Lin, Y. W. & Tsai, M. L. (2006) Culture and the Kindergarten Curriculum in Taiwan. Early Child Development and Care, 123(1), 157–165,  https://doi.org/10.1080/0300443961230111.
  20. Mori, Y., & Calder, T. (2017). The role of parental support and family variables in L1 and L2 vocabulary development of Japanese Heritage Language students in the United States. Foreign Language Annals, 50(4), 754–775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Nicoladis, E., Rose, A., & Foursha-Stevenson, C. (2010). Thinking for speaking and cross-linguistic transfer in preschool bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13(3), 345–370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Thordardottir, E. (2011). The relationship between bilingual exposure and vocabulary development. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15(4), 426–445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Zhang, H. (2016). Early Language input and later reading development in Chinese as heritage language (CHL) learners. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 26(3), 437–448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd. 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Monash UniversityMelbourneAustralia

Personalised recommendations