Advertisement

The Stuff that Minds Are Made of

  • V. Srinivasa Chakravarthy
Chapter

Abstract

So far we have endeavored to demystify a few things about the brain. How is sensory information organized as maps in the brain? How are memories taken through a series of resting stages through the brain? How are fears processed and integrated with higher cognitive processes in the brain? How do simple cellular level changes underlie complex learning phenomena? Most of these processes can be shown to be implemented by networks of neurons passing neural spike signals among themselves. The circuits involved might vary, the precise nature of the signals might vary, but the broad framework remains the same and this is one of the celebrated successes of contemporary neuroscience. Using the scientific method, and the intimidating and extravagant repertoire of contemporary neuroscience methods and materials, from single neuron recordings to gene knockouts, it is possible to present a scientific theory of how brain works.

References

  1. Baars, B. J. (2002). The conscious access hypothesis: Origins and recent evidence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(1), 47–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baars, B. J. (2005). Global workspace theory of consciousness: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of human experience? Progress in Brain Research, 150, 45–53.Google Scholar
  3. Bach-y-Rita, P. (1972). Brain mechanisms in sensory substitution. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  4. Barker, A. T., Jalinous, R., & Freeston, I. L. (1985). Non-invasive magnetic stimulation of human motor cortex. The Lancet, 1(8437), 1106–1107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Blake, R., & Tong, F. (2008). Bicular rivalry. Scholarpedia, 3(12), 1578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Botvinick, M., & Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber hands “feel” touch that eyes see. Nature, 391, 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brindley, G. S., & Lewin, W. S. (1968). The sensations produced by electrical stimulation of the visual cortex. The Journal of Physiology, 196, 479–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Crick, F. (1995). The astonishing hypothesis: The scientific search for the soul, scribner reprint edition. Simon & Schuster Adult Publishing Group.Google Scholar
  9. Damasio, A. R. (1989). Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition, 33, 25–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Cohen, L., Le Bihan, D., Mangin, J.-F., Poline, J.-B., et al. (2001). Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming. Nature Neuroscience, 4(7), 752.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dennet, D. C. (1993). Consciousness explained. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
  12. Descartes, R. (1641/1984). Meditations on first philosophy. In The philosophical writings of René Descartes (Vol. 2, pp. 1–62) (J. Cottingham, R. Stoothoff, & D. Murdoch, Trans.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  13. Edelman, G., & Tononi, G. (2000). A universe of consciousness: How matter becomes imagination. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  14. Freeman, W. J. (1991). The physiology of perception. Scientific American, 264, 78–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gray, C. M., & Singer, W. (1989). Stimulus-specific neuronal oscillations in orientation columns of cat visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 86, 1698–1702.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Holt, J. (1991). How children learn (pp. 18–20). London: Penguin Publishers.Google Scholar
  17. James, W. (1890), The principles of psychology (p. 136). Cambridge: Harvard University Press. 1983 paperback, ISBN 0-674-70625-0 (combined edition, 1328 pages).Google Scholar
  18. Kornhuber, H. H., & Deecke, L. (1990). Readiness for movement—The Bereitschaftspotential-story. Current Contents Life Sciences, 33(4), 14.Google Scholar
  19. Lee, S.-H., Blake, R., & Heeger, D. J. (2005). Travelling waves of activity in primary visual cortex during binocular rivalry. Nature Neuroscience, 8(1), 22–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Lee, S.-H., Blake, R., & Heeger, D. J. (2007). Hierarchy of cortical responses underlying binocular rivalry. Nature Neuroscience, 10(8), 1048–1054.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Libet, B. (1965). Cortical activation in conscious and unconscious experience. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 9, 77–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Libet, B. (1981). The experimental evidence for subjective referral of a sensory experience backwards in time: Reply to PS Churchland. Philosophy of Science, 48, 182–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Llinas, R., & Ribary, U. (2001). Consciousness and the brain: The thalamocortical dialogue in health and disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 929, 166–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Logothetis, N., & Schal, J. (1989). Neuronal correlates of subjective visual perception. Science, 245(4919), 761–763.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. McFadden, J. (2002). The conscious electromagnetic information (Cemi) field theory: The hard problem made easy? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 9(8), 45–60.Google Scholar
  26. Melloni, L., Molina, C., Pena, M., Torres, D., Singer, W., & Rodriguez, E. (2007). Synchronization of neural activity across cortical areas correlates with conscious perception. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(11), 2858–2865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Penfield, W. (1958). The excitable cortex in conscious man. Liverpool: Liverpool University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Pockett, S. (2000). The nature of consciousness. Writer Club Press, Lincoln, Nebraska.Google Scholar
  29. Porta, J. B. (1593). De refractione. Optices parte. Libri novem. Naples: Salviani.Google Scholar
  30. Ramachandran, V. S. (1992, May). Blind spots. Scientific American, pp. 86–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Ramachandran, V. S., & Gregory, R. L. (1991). Perceptual filling in of artificially induced scotomas in human vision. Nature, 350, 699–702.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Schmaltz, T. (2002). Nicolas Malebranche. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Summer 2002 Edition). http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2002/entries/malebranche/.
  33. Singer, W. (2007). Binding by synchrony. Scholarpedia, 2(12), 1657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Stapp, H. (1993). Mind, matter and quantum mechanics. Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Tecchio, F., Babiloni, C., Zappasodi, F., Vecchio, F., Pizzella, V., Romani, G. L., et al. (2003). Gamma synchronization in human primary somatosensory cortex as revealed by somatosensory evoked neuromagnetic fields. Brain Research, 986(1–2), 63–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Walter, W. G. (1963). Presentation to the Osler society. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  37. Watson, J. B. quote: Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review, 20, 158–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Weiskrantz, L. (1988). Some contributions of neuropsychology of vision and memory to the problem of consciousness. In A. Marcel & E. Bisiach (Eds.), Consciousness in contemporary science (pp. 183–199).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Weiskrantz, L. (1997). Consciousness lost and found. A neuropsychological exploration. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  40. Weiskrantz, L. (2007). Blindsight. Scholarpedia, 2(4), 3047.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Wheatstone, C. (1838). Contributions to the physiology of vision. Part the first. On some remarkable, and hitherto unobserved, phænomena of binocular vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 128, 371–394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd. 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Indian Institute of Technology MadrasChennaiIndia

Personalised recommendations