Advertisement

Toward an Understanding of Phonological Awareness

  • Hugh W. Catts
  • Kim A. Wilcox
  • Carla Wood-Jackson
  • Linda S. Larrivee
  • Victoria G. Scott
Part of the NATO ASI Series book series (ASID, volume 87)

Abstract

Numerous studies have documented a relationship between phonological awareness and reading achievement. Despite this work, phonological awareness and its development are not well understood. This chapter examines phonological awareness, first by considering how it has been measured, and then, by studying factors related to performance on a frequently used measure of phonological awareness. A review of the literature showed that numerous tasks have been used to measure children’s awareness of phonemes. These can be divided into phoneme segmentation, phoneme blending, and sound comparison tasks. It is proposed that performance on phoneme segmentation and phoneme blending tasks is heavily influenced by experience with an alphabetic writing system. An investigation of a commonly used sound comparison task, the oddity task, suggested that performance on this task was dependent in part on chidren’s perceptual and memory abilities.

Keywords

Phonological Awareness Speech Sound Learn Disability Reading Acquisition Awareness Task 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bloomfield, L. (1933). Language. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.Google Scholar
  2. Bentin, S., and Leshem, H. (1993). On the interaction between phonological awareness and reading acquisition: It’s a two way street. Annals of Dyslexia, 43, 125–148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bradley, L., and Bryant, P. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read: A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bradley, L., and Bryant, P. (1985). Rhyme and reason in reading and spelling. Ann Arbor, MI.: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
  5. Burns, E., and Ward, W. (1978). Categorical perception-phenomenon or epiphenomenon: Evidence from experiments in the perception of melodic musical intervals. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 63, 456–468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Byrne, B., and Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1990). Acquiring the alphabetic principle: A case for teaching recognition of phoneme identity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 805–812.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Byrne, B., and Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1991). Evaluation of a program to teach phonemic awareness to young children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 451–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Byrne, B., and Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1993). Recognition of phoneme invariance by beginning readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 5, 315–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Byrne, B., and Ledz, J., (1983). Phonological awareness in reading-disabled adults. Australian Journal of Psychology, 35, 185–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Carney, A., Widin, G., and Viemeister, N. (1977). Noncategorical perception of stop consonants differing in VOT. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 62. 961–970.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Carts, H. (1993). The relationship between speech-language impairments and reading disabilities. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 948–958.Google Scholar
  12. Catts, H., and Scott, V. (1994). Measuring phonological awareness. Working paper of the Kansas Early Childhood Research Institute. University of Kansas.Google Scholar
  13. Chall, J., Roswell, F., and Blumenthal, S. (1963). Auditory blending ability: A factor of success in beginning reading. Reading Teacher, 17, 113–118.Google Scholar
  14. Cossu, G., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I., Katz, L., and Tolar, G. (1988). Awareness of phonological segments and reading ability in Italian children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Ehri, L. (1989). The development of spelling knowledge and its role in reading acquisition and reading disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 356–364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Ehri, L., Wilce, L., and Taylor, B. (1988). Children’s categorization of short vowels in words and the influence of spellings. In K. Stanovich (Ed.), Children’s reading and the development of phonological awareness. (pp. 149–177 ). Detroit: Wayne State University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Eilers, R., and Minifie, F. (1975). Fricative discrimination in early infancy. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 18, 158–167.Google Scholar
  18. Eimas, P. (1975). Auditory and phonetic coding of the cues for speech: Discrimination of the [r-1] distinction by young infants. Perception and Psychophysics, 18, 341–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Felton, R., and Brown, I. (1990). Phonological processing as predictors of specific reading skills in at risk for reading failure. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 39–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Foster, K., Erickson, G., Foster, D., Brinkman, D., and Torgesen. J. (1994). Computer administered instruction in phonological awareness: Evaluation of the DaisyQuest Program. Journal of Research and Development in Education, 17, 126–137.Google Scholar
  21. Fox, B., and Routh, D. (1975). Analyzing language into words, syllables, and phonemes: A developmental study. Journal of Psychological Research, 4, 331–342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Frances, W., and Kucera, H. (1982). Frequency analysis of English usage: Lexicon and grammar. Boston: Houghton Miffl in Company.Google Scholar
  23. Golinkoff, R. (1978). Critique: Phonemic awareness skills and reading achievement. In F. Murray and J. Pikulski (Eds.), The acquisition of reading: Cognition, linguistic, and perceptual prerequisites (pp. 23–41 ). Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
  24. Juel, C., Griffith, P., and Gough, P. (1986). Acquisition of literacy: A longitudinal study of children in first and second grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 243–255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kamhi, A., and Catts, H. (1986). Toward an understanding of developmental language and reading disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 51, 337–347.Google Scholar
  26. Kuhl, P. (1980). Perceptual constancy for speech-sound categories in early infancy. In G. Yeni-Komshian, J. Kavanagh, and C. Ferguson (Eds.), Child Phonology: Vol. 2. Perception (pp. 41–66 ). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  27. Lenchner, O., Gerber, M., and Routh, D. (1990). Phonological awareness tasks as predictors of decoding ability: Beyond segmentation. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23, 240–247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Lewis, B. and Freebairn, L. (1992). Residual effects of preschool phonology disorders in grade school, adolescence, and adulthood. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 819–831.Google Scholar
  29. Lewkowicz, M. (1980). Phonemic awareness training: What to teach and how to teach it. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 686–700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Liberman, A., Cooper, F., Shankweiler, D., and Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review, 24, 431–461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Liberman, I.Y.,and Shankweiler, D. (1979). Speech, the alphabet, and teaching to read. In L. Resnick and P. Weaver (eds.) Theory and practice of early reading. (vol 2, pp. 109–132 ). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  32. Liberman, I.Y., and Shankweiler, D, (1985). Phonology and the problems of learning to read and write. Remedial and Special Education, 6, 8–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Liberman, I.Y., Shankweiler, D., Fischer, F., and Carter, B. (1974). Explicit syllable and phoneme segmentation in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18, 201–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Lindamood, C. and Lindamood, P. (1975). The A.D.D. program: Auditory discrimination in depth. (2nd ed.) Allen, TX: DLM, Inc.Google Scholar
  35. Lundberg, I., Frost, J., and Petersen, O. (1988). Effects of an extensive program far stimulating phonological awareness in preschool children. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 263–284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Lundberg, I., and Hoien, T. (1991). Initial enabling knowledge and skills in reading acquisition: Print awareness and phonological segmentation. In D. Sawyer and B. Fox (Eds.), Phonological awareness in reading: The evolution of current perspectives (pp. 73–95 ). New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Lundberg, I., Olofsson, A., and Wall. S. (1980). Reading and spelling skills in the first school years predicted from phonemic awareness skills in kindergarten. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 21, 159–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Maclean, M., Bryant, P., and Bradley, L. (1988). Rhymes, nursery rhymes and reading in early childhood. In K. Stanovich (Ed.), Children’s reading and the development of phonological awareness (pp. 11–37 ). Detroit: Wayne State University Press.Google Scholar
  39. Mann, V. (1993). Phoneme awareness and future reading ability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26, 259–269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Mann, V., and Liberman, I (1984). Phonological awareness and verbal short term memory. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 17, 592–599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Mann, V., Tobin, P., and Wilson, R. (1987). Measuring phonological awareness through the invented spellings of kindergarten children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 365–391.Google Scholar
  42. Marsh, G., and Mineo, R. (1977). Training preschool children to recognize phonemes in words. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 748–753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Morais, J. (1991). Phonological awareness: A bridge between language and literacy. In D.J. Sawyer and BJ. Fox (Eds.), Phonological awareness in reading. (pp. 31–71 ). New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Morais, J., Bertelson, P., Cary, L., and Alegria, J. (1986). Literacy training and speech segmentation. Cognition, 24, 45–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Morals, J., Cary, L., Alegria, J., and Bertelson, P. (1979). Does awareness of speech as a sequence of phones arise spontaneously? Cognition, 7, 323–331.Google Scholar
  46. Morse, P. (1972). The discrimination of speech and nonspeech stimuli in early infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 14, 477–492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Näslund, J., and Schneider, W. (1991). Longitudinal effects of verbal ability, memory capacity, and phonological awareness in reading performance. European Journal of Psychology, 6, 375–392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Nesdale, A., Herriman, M., and Tunmer, W. (1984). Phonological awareness in children. In W. Tunmer, C. Pratt, and M. Herriman (Eds.), Metalinguistic awareness in children: Theory, research and implications (pp. 36–54 ). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  49. Olson, R., Wise, B., Conners, F., Rack, J., and Fulker, D. (1989). Specific deficits in component reading and language skills: Genetic and experimental influences. Journal of Learning Disability, 22, 339–348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Perfetti, C., Beck, I., Bell, L., and Hughes, C. (1988). Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal: A longitudinal study of first grade children. In K. Stanovich (Ed.), Children’s reading and the development of phonological awareness. (pp. 39–75 ). Detroit: Wayne State University Press.Google Scholar
  51. Perin, D. (1983). Phonemic segmentation in spelling. British Journal of Psychology, 74, 129–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Read, C., and Ruyter, L. (1985). Reading and spelling skills in adults of low literacy. Remedial and Special Education, 6, 43–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Read, C., Zhang, Y., Nie, H., and Ding, B. (1986). The ability to manipulate speech sound depends on knowing alphabetic reading. Cognition, 24, 21–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Rosner, J., and Simon, D. (1971). The auditory analysis test: An initial report. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 4, 40–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Sawyer, D. (1987). Test of awareness of language segements. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Sawyer, D.J., and Fox, BJ. (1991). Phonological awareness in reading: The evolution of current perspectives New York Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  56. Share, D., Jorm, A., MacLean, R., and Matthews, R. (1984). Sources of individual differences in reading acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 6, 1309–1324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Shriberg, L., and Kent, R. (1982). Clinical phonetics. New York: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
  58. Singh, S., and Woods, D. (1971). Perceptual structure of 12 American English vowels. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 46, 1861–1866.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Singh, S., Woods., and Becker, G. (1972). Perceptual structure of 22 prevocalic English consonants. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 52, 1698–1711.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Skjelfjord, V. (1987). Phonemic segmentation: An important subskill in learning to read: 1. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 31, 41–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Stahl, S., and Murray, B. (1994). Defining phonological awareness and its relationship to early reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 221–234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Stanovich, K. (Ed.). (1988). Children’s reading and the development of phonological awareness. Detroit: Wayne State University Press.Google Scholar
  63. Stanovich, K., Cunningham, A., and Cramer, B. (1984). Assessing phonological awareness in kindergarten children: Issues of task comparability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 175–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Stuart, M., and Coltheart, M. (1988). Does reading develop in a sequence of stages? Cognition, 30, 139–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Torgesen, J., and Bryant, B. (1994). Test of phonological awareness. Austin, Texas: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
  66. Torgesen, J., and Morgan, S. (1990). Phonological synthesis tasks: A developmental, functional, and componential analysis. In L. Swanson and B. Keogh (Eds.) Learning disabilities: Theoretical and research issues (pp. 263–276 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  67. Torgesen, J., Morgan, S., and Davis, C. (1992). Effects of two types of phonological awareness training on word learning in kindergarten children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 364–370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Torgesen, J., Wagner, R., Balthazar, M., Davis, C., Morgan, S., Simmons, K., Stage, S., and Zips, F. (1989). Development and individual differences in performance on phonological synthesis tasks. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47, 491–505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Torgesen, J., Wagner, R., and Rashotte, C. (1994). Longitudinal studies of phonological processing and reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 276–286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Tornéus, M. (1984). Phonological awareness and reading: A chicken and egg problem? Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 1346–1358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Treiman, R., and Baron, J. (1981). Segmental analysis ability: Development and relationship to reading. In G.E. MacKinnon and T.G. Waller (Eds.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice, (Vol. 3 ), (pp. 159–196 ). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  72. Treiman, R., and Baron, J. (1983). Phonemic-analysis training helps children benefit from spelling-sound rules. Memory and Cognition, 11, 382–389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Treiman, R., and Breaux, A. (1982). Common phoneme and overall similarity relations among spoken syllables: Their use by children and adults. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 11, 569–598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Treiman, R., and Zukowski, A. (1991). Levels of phonological awareness. In S. Brady and D. Shankweiler (Fels.) Phonological processes in literacy (pp. 67–83 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  75. Tunmer, W., and Nesdale, A. (1985). Phonemic segmentation skill and beginning reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 417–427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Tunmer, W., Pratt, C., and Herriman, M. (Eds.) (1984). Metalinguistic awareness in children: Theory, research and implications. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  77. Vellutino, F., and Scanlon, D. (1988). Phonological coding, phonological awareness and reading ability: Evidence from a longitudinal and experimental study. In K. Stanovich (Ed.), Children’s reading and the development of phonological awareness (pp. 77–119 ). Detroit: Wayne State University.Google Scholar
  78. Wagner, R., and Torgesen, J. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Wang, M., and Bilger, R. (1973). Consonant confusions in noise: A study of perceptual features. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 54, 1248–1266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Wallach, L., Wallach, M., Dozier, M., and Kaplan, N. (1977). Poor children learning to read do not have trouble with auditory discrimination but do have trouble with phoneme recognition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 36–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Wimmer, H., Landerl, K. Linortner, R., and Hummer, P. (1991). The relationship of phonological awareness to reading acquisition: More consequence than precondition but still important. Cognition, 40, 219–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Yopp, H. (1988). The validity and reliability of phonemic awareness tests. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 159–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Zifcak, M. (1981). Phonological awareness and reading acquisition. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 6, 117–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 1997

Authors and Affiliations

  • Hugh W. Catts
    • 1
  • Kim A. Wilcox
    • 1
  • Carla Wood-Jackson
    • 1
  • Linda S. Larrivee
    • 1
  • Victoria G. Scott
    • 1
  1. 1.Speech-Language-Hearing: Sciences and DisordersUniversity of KansasLawrenceUSA

Personalised recommendations