The Influence of Emotional Deprivation on Growth and Behaviour

  • W. Croughs
Part of the Boerhaave Series for Postgraduate Medical Education book series (BSPM, volume 6)

Abstract

The first studies of emotional deprivation concerned children living in an institution (1–7). Spitz and Wolf (7) studied children in a foundling home where the infants lived in cubicles up to 15 to 18 months of age, completely separated from each other. Their visual radius and locomotion were extremely restricted. During their first few months these infants were breastfed by their mothers. Permanent separation from the mother took place predominantly in the sixth month. After removal of the mother, many of these children developed a syndrome that the authors called anaclitic depression. Beginning with apprehension, sadness, and weeping, the syndrome continued into rejection, withdrawal, loss of appetite, refusal to eat, loss of weight, loss of interest in the outside world, stereotyped movements, dejection, retardation, and finally a condition that could only be described as stuporous. In spite of the fact that hygiene and precautions against contagion were impeccable, the children showed marked susceptibility to infection and illness of every kind. During a follow-up period of two years this group showed a mortality of 37 per cent. Of 21 surviving children still at the institution, only 2 fell within the height range of a normal two-year-old child (6). Only 5 out of 21 were able to walk unassisted. Mental and speech development were extremely retarded. The authors also studied another institution where the infants were less isolated and were cared for by their mothers (delinquent girls). In a number of cases the mother was temporarily removed somewhere between the sixth and eighth month. Some of these children also developed the syndrome of anaclitic depression. After restoration of the love object following approximately three months of absence, the syndrome disappeared. This was in contrast to the cases of longer-lasting separation in the foundling home, where intervention was no longer effective and the syndrome appeared to become irreversible. During a follow-up period of one year not a single child died and motor and mental development were within normal limits. As regards the aetiology of the syndrome, Durfee and Wolf (1), who performed an investigation of institutional care of infants in 1933, already stressed two important factors that were responsible for the psychological injury suffered by these infants: lack of stimulation and absence of the mother. Spitz and Wolf (7) also stressed the importance of free locomotion in the second half-year of life, which they considered to be the most vulnerable age. At that age, motor activity can be used for an attempt to seek replacement for the lost love object. Moreover, it can be argued that in the syndrome of anaclitic depression the acute separation of mother and child is more harmful than mere absence of the mother.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. 1.
    Durfee, H. & Wolf, K., Anstaltspflege und Entwicklung im ersten Lebensjahr. Zeitschr. Kinderforschung 42, 3 (1933).Google Scholar
  2. 2.
    Gesell, A. & Amatruda, T., Environmental retardation. Developmental diagnosis. New-York 1941.Google Scholar
  3. 3.
    Bakwin, H., Loneliness in infants. Amer. J. Dis. Child. 63, 30 (1942).Google Scholar
  4. 4.
    Bakwin, H., Emotional deprivation in infants. J. Pediat. 35, 512 (1949).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    Spitz, R. A., Hospitalism. Psychoanal. Stud. Child 1, 53 (1945).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Spitz, R. A., Hospitalism. A follow-up report on investigation described in Vol. I, 1945. Psychoanal. Stud. Child 2, 113 (1946).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. 7.
    Spitz, R. A. & Wolf, K., Anaclitic depression. Psychoanal. Stud. Child 2, 313 (1946).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    Lowrey, L. G., Personality distortion and early institutional care. Am. J. Orthopsychiat. 10, 576 (1940).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    Bowlby, J., The influence of early environment in the development of neurosis and neurotic character. Int. J. Psychoanal. 21, 1 (1940).Google Scholar
  10. 10.
    Bowlby, J., Maternal care and mental health. World Health Organisation monograph series, Geneva 1952.Google Scholar
  11. 11.
    Goldfarb, W., The effects of early institutional care on adolescent personality. J. Exp. Educ. 12, 2 (1943).Google Scholar
  12. 12.
    Goldfarb, W., The effects of early institutional care on adolescent personality (Graphic Rorschach data). Child Developm. 14, 4 (1943).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Goldfarb, W., The effects of early institutional care on adolescent personality. Am. J. Orthopsychiat. 14, 3 (1944).Google Scholar
  14. 14.
    Goldfarb, W., Psychological privation in infancy and subsequent adjustment. Am. J. Orthopsychiat. 15, 247 (1945).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. 15.
    Goldfarb, W., Effects of psychological deprivation in infancy and subsequent stimulation. Am. J. Psychiat. 102, 18 (1945).Google Scholar
  16. 16.
    Fried, R. & Mayer, M. F., Socio-emotional factors accounting for growth failure in children in an institution. J. Pediat. 33, 444 (1948).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. 17.
    Widdowson, E. M., Mental contentment and physical growth. Lancet I, 1316 (1951).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. 18.
    Talbot, N. B., Sobel, E. H., Burke, B. S., Lindemann, E. & Kaufman, S. H., Dwar-fism in healthy children: its possible relation to emotional, nutritional and endocrine disturbance. NewEngl. J. Med. 236, 783 (1947).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. 19.
    Coleman, R. W. & Provence, S., Emotional retardation (hospitalism) in infants living in families. Pediatrics 19, 285 (1957).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. 20.
    Elmer, E., Failure to thrive. Role of the mother. Pediatrics 25, 717 (1960).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. 21.
    Patton, R. G. & Gardner, L. I., Influence of family environment on growth; the syndrome of maternal deprivation. Pediatrics 30, 957 (1962).Google Scholar
  22. 22.
    Patton, R. G. & Gardner, L. I., Growth failure in maternal deprivation. Springfield Ill.) 1963.Google Scholar
  23. 23.
    Leonard, M. F., Rhymes, J. P. & Solnit, A. J., Failure to thrive in infants. Amer. J. Dis. Child. III, 600 (1966).Google Scholar
  24. 24.
    Powell, G. F., Brasel. J. A. & Blizzard, R. M., Emotional deprivation and growth retardation simulating idiopathic hypopituitarism. I. Clinical evaluation of the syndrome. New. Engl. J. Med. 276, 1270 (1967).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. 25.
    Silver, H. K. & Finkelstein, M., Deprivation dwarfism. J. Pediat. 70, 317 (1967).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. 26.
    Elmer, E., Gregg, E. S. & Ellison, P., Late results of failure to thrive syndrome. Clin. Ped. 8, 584 (1969).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. 27.
    Krieger, I. & Sargent, D. A., A postural sign in the sensory deprivation syndrome in infants. J. Pediat. 70, 332 (1967).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. 28.
    Whitten, Ch. F., Pettit, M. G. & Fischoff, F., Evidence that growth failure from maternal deprivation is secondary to undereating. J. Amer, med. Ass. 209, 1675 (1969).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. 29.
    Blodgett, F. M., Growth retardation related to maternal deprivation. In: Modern perspectives in child development. Solnit, A. J. & Provence, S. A. (eds.), New-York 1963.Google Scholar
  30. 30.
    Glaser, H. H., Heagarty, M. C., Bullard, D. M. & Pivchik, E. C., Physical and psychological development of children with early failure to thrive. J. Pediat. 73, 690 (1968).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. 31.
    Shaheen, E., Alexander, D., Tuskowsky, M. & Barbero, G., Failure to thrive — a retrospective profile. Clin. Ped. 7, 255 (1968).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. 32.
    Elmer, E. & Gregg, G. S., Developmental characteristics of abused children. Pediatrics 40, 596 (1967).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. 33.
    Powell, G. F., Brasel, J. A., Raid, S. & Blizzard, R. S., Emotional deprivation and growth retardation simulating idopathic hypopituitarism. II. Endocrinologie evaluation of the syndrome. New Engl. J. Med. 276, 1276 (1967).Google Scholar
  34. 34.
    Pimstone, B. L. Wittman, W., Hansen, J. D. L. & Murray, P., Growth hormone and kwashiorkor. Lancet II, 779 (1966).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. 35.
    Pimstone, B. L., Barbezat, G., Hansen, J. D. L. & Murray, P., Studies on growth hormone secretion in protein-calorie malnutrition. Amer. J. Clin. Nutr. 21, 482 (1968).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. 36.
    Graham, G. G., Cordano, A., Blizzard, R. M. & Cheek, D. B., Infantile malnutrition: changes in body composition during rehabilitation. Pediat. Res. 3, 579 (1969).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. 37.
    Milner, R. D. G., Hormonal and metabolic interrelationships in malnutrition. Pediat. Res. 4, 213 (1970).Google Scholar
  38. 38.
    Croughs, W. & Fedder, J. J., Psychogene groeistoornis en battered child syndroom bij drie kinderen. Ned. T. Geneesk. 114, 672 (1970).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. 39.
    Van Wieringen, J. C., Wafelbakker, F., Verbrugge, H. P. & de Haas, J. H., Groei-diagrammen Nederland 1965. Groningen 1968.Google Scholar
  40. 40.
    Tanner, J. M. & Whitehouse, R. H., Standards for subcutaneous fat in British children. Brit. med. J. I, 446 (1962).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. 41.
    Keet, M. P., Hansen, J. D. L. & Truswell, A. S., Are skinfold measurements of value in the assessment of suboptimal nutrition in young children? Pediatrics 45, 965 (1970).PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Leiden University Press, Leiden, The Netherlands 1971

Authors and Affiliations

  • W. Croughs

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations