Lexical Availability of Basic and Advanced Semantic Categories in English L1 and English L2

Chapter
Part of the Educational Linguistics book series (EDUL, volume 17)

Abstract

The current investigation used lexical availability to assess the performance of Chilean university students, advanced English (L2) learners in comparison with English native speakers (L1) in basic (‘Body parts’, ‘Food and drink’), and advanced (‘Terrorism and crime’, ‘Health and medicine’) semantic categories. Three analyses were conducted looking at number of words produced, lexical availability values, and correlations between L1 and L2 speakers in all four semantic categories. The results of the first analysis showed that L1 outperformed L2 speakers and that basic categories were more productive than advanced categories regarding number of words produced. The second analysis showed no group effect, but a significant effect of semantic category, with basic showing higher lexical availability than advanced categories. The last analysis revealed strong correlations between L1 and L2 speakers in all semantic categories, with stronger correlations for basic than advanced categories. However, the most significant finding in this study is that both groups retrieved a greater number of words for basic semantic categories than for advanced semantic categories which seem to point to similar patterns in the organization of the available lexicons of L1 and L2 speakers.

Keywords

Native Speaker Semantic Category Basic Category Mental Lexicon Word Production 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Notes

Acknowledgments

This study was partially supported by Proyecto Fondecyt 1050598 (2005). We would like to thank Linda Salter and Paul Salter for their collaboration in collecting the data from the English speakers. We would also like to thank Prof. Lilian Gómez who allowed us to take part of her teaching time in order to conduct the lexical availability test at the University of Concepción. Many thanks to the students from the Royal School Haslemere, and the students from the University of Concepcion, who participated in the study. Finally, we would also like to thank Mariya Bistrina for proofreading this chapter.

References

  1. Anwar Amer, A.A. 1986. Semantic field theory and vocabulary teaching. English Teaching Forum 24: 30–31.Google Scholar
  2. Baayen, R.H., R. Piepenbrock, and H. van Rijn. 1993. The CELEX lexical database (CD-ROM). Philadelphia: Linguistic Data Consortium.Google Scholar
  3. Balota, D.A., and J.I. Chumbley. 1984. Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access? The role of word frequency in the neglected decision state. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 133: 283–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Balota, D.A., M.J. Cortese, S.D. Sergent-Marshall, D.H. Spieler, and M.J. Yap. 2004. Visual word recognition of single-syllable words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 133: 283–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Barry, C., C.M. Morrison, and A.W. Ellis. 1997. Naming the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures: Effects of age of acquisition, frequency and name agreement. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 50A: 560–585.Google Scholar
  6. Bates, E., C. Burani, S. D’Amico, and L. Barca. 2001. Word reading and picture naming in Italian. Memory and Cognition 29: 986–999.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brysbaert, M., and B. New. 2009. Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods 41: 977–990.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Caramazza, A. 1997. How many levels of processing are there in lexical access? Cognitive Neuropsychology 14: 177–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Carroll, J.B., and M.N. White. 1973. Word frequency and age-of-acquisition as determiners of picture-naming latency. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 25: 85–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Centre International d’études pédagogiques. 2012. Diplôme d’études en langue française DELF and Diplôme approfondi de langue française DALF. http://www.ciep.fr/delfdalf/index.php. Accessed 5 Jan 2012.
  11. Channell, J. 1990. Vocabulary acquisition and the mental lexicon. In Meaning and lexicography, ed. J. Tomasczyk and B. Lewandowska-Tomasczyk, 21–30. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
  12. Costa, A. 2005. Lexical access in bilingual production. In Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches, ed. J.F. Kroll and A.M.B. De Groot, 308–325. Cary: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  13. Costa, A., M. Miozzo, and A. Caramazza. 1999. Lexical selection in bilinguals: Do words in the bilingual’s two lexicons compete for selection? Journal of Memory and Language 41: 365–397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Cuetos, F., A.W. Ellis, and B. Alvarez. 1999. Naming times for the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures in Spanish. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers 31: 650–658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Cuetos, F., G. Aguado, C. Izura, and A.W. Ellis. 2002. Aphasic naming in Spanish: Predictors and errors. Brain and Language 82: 344–365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Cycowicz, Y.M., D. Friedman, M. Rothstein, and J.G. Snodgrass. 1997. Picture naming by young children: Norms for name agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 65: 171–237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. De Bot, K. 1992. A bilingual production model: Levelt’s speaking model adapted. Applied Linguistics 13: 1–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dijkstra, T. 2005. Bilingual visual word recognition and lexical access. In Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches, ed. J.F. Kroll and A.M.B. De Groot, 179–201. Cary: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Echeverría, M.S., P. Urzúa, and I. Figueroa. 2005. Dispogen II. Programa computacional para el análisis de la disponibilidad léxica. Concepción: Universidad de Concepción.Google Scholar
  20. Educational Testing Service. 2012. Test of English for international communication TOEIC. http://www.ets.org/toeic. Accessed 5 Jan 2012.
  21. Ellis, A.W., and C.M. Morrison. 1998. Real age of acquisition effects in lexical retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 24: 515–523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Ferreira, R.A. 2006. Disponibilidad léxica en inglés como lengua materna e inglés como lengua extranjera: Estudio del léxico disponible desde un enfoque psicolingüístico. Unpublished dissertation. Concepción: University of Concepción.Google Scholar
  23. Ferreira, R.A. 2011. Learning the meaning of new words: Behavioural and neuroimaging evidence. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. York: University of York.Google Scholar
  24. Ferreira, R.A., and M.S. Echeverría. 2010. Redes semánticas en el léxico disponible de inglés L1 e inglés LE. Onomazein 21: 133–153.Google Scholar
  25. Hernández-Muñoz, N., C. Izura, and A.W. Ellis. 2006. Cognitive aspects of lexical availability. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 18: 730–755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Instituto Cervantes. 2012. Diplomas de español como lengua extranjera. http://diplomas.cervantes.es/es/informacion/inscripcion_fechas_examen_dele.html. Accessed 5 Jan 2012.
  27. Kucĕra, H., and W.N. Francis. 1967. Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence: Brown University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Larochelle, S., and H. Pineau. 1994. Determinants of response times in the category verification task. Journal of Memory and Language 33: 796–823.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Levelt, W.J.M. 1989. Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  30. López-Chávez, J., and C. Strassburguer-Frías. 1991. Un modelo para el cálculo del índice de disponibilidad léxica individual. In La enseñanza del español como lengua materna, ed. H. López-Morales, 91–112. Río Piedras: Universidad de Puerto Rico.Google Scholar
  31. López-Morales, H. 2012. EL Proyecto Panhispánico. http://www.dispolex.com/. Accessed 12 June 2013.
  32. Malt, B.C., and E.E. Smith. 1982. The role of familiarity in determining typicality. Memory and Cognition 10: 69–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. McCarthy, M., and F. O’Dell. 2002. English vocabulary in use: Advanced. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  34. Microsoft Corporation. 2007. Windows XP note block. http://windows.microsoft.com/es-XL/windows/products/windows-xp. Accessed 3 Apr 2007.
  35. Monaghan, J., and A.W. Ellis. 2002. What, exactly, interacts with spelling-sound consistency in word naming? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 28: 183–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Morrison, C.M., and A.W. Ellis. 2000. Real age of acquisition effects in word naming and lexical decision. British Journal of Psychology 91: 167–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Poulisse, N. 1999. Slips of the tongue: Speech errors in first and second language production. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
  38. Pye, G. 2002. Vocabulary in practice 1. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  39. Redman, S. 2001. English vocabulary in use: Pre-intermediate and intermediate. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  40. Richards, C.J., and C. Sandy. 2008. Passages student’s book, 2nd ed. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  41. Richards, C.J., J. Hull, and S. Proctor. 2005. Interchange, 3rd ed. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  42. Snodgrass, J.G., and T. Yuditsky. 1996. Naming times for the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers 28: 516–536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. The Math Works Inc. 2005. MATLAB version 7.1. Natick, Microsoft Corporation.Google Scholar
  44. Thorndike, E.L. 1921. A teacher’s word book of 20,000 words. New York: Teacher’s College.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Departamento de Lenguas. Facultad de EducaciónUniversidad Católica de la Santísima ConcepciónConcepciónChile

Personalised recommendations