Advertisement

Autoscopy: Disrupted Self in Neuropsychiatric Disorders and Anomalous Conscious States

  • Aaron L. Mishara
Chapter

Abstract

Autoscopy is a loosely related complex of experiences in which one sees (or experiences) a “double” as external to one’s current vantage point. The phenomenology of autoscopy provides an alternative to current “models” of self in cognitive science. After years of silence on matters such as consciousness and self, cognitive science and neuroscience have now swung in the opposite direction, and claim to be able to experimentally study these topics, often in an oversimplified manner. These approaches uncritically confuse representational content about self or self-awareness in self-referential processing, i.e., having a self (a self-enclosed entity), with being a self, prospectively open to its own (yet-to-be-known) future.1 Ignoring this difference has led to an industry of philosophical essays and neuroimaging studies that claim to access the first-person perspective when only able to access higher order self-referential judgments (for critical reviews, see Fuchs 2006; Legrand et al. 2003; Mishara 2007b). A similar confusion prevails in current approaches to classify types of autoscopy in the search for its underlying cognitive-neural mechanisms.2 Due to its descriptive method and resulting theoretical framework, phenomenology is in a unique position to contribute to the study of human self and its disruption in neuropsychatric disorders.

Keywords

Body Image Neuropsychiatric Disorder Inferior Parietal Lobe Phenomenological Analysis Body Schema 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Ach N (1905) Über die Willenstätigkeit und das Denken. Vandenhoeck & Rubprecht, GöttingenGoogle Scholar
  2. Arenz D (2001) Heautoskopie. Doppelgängerphänomen und seltene Halluzination der eigenen Gestalt. Nervenarzt 72:376–379Google Scholar
  3. Arzy S, Seeck M, Ortigue S, Spinelli L, Blanke O (2006) Induction of an illusory shadow person. Nature 443:287CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Battelli L, Pascual-Leone A, Cavanagh P (2007) The ‘when’ pathway of the right parietal lobe. Trends Cogn Sci 11:204–210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Binswanger L (1957) Schizophrenie. Neske, PfullingenGoogle Scholar
  6. Binswanger L (1965) Wahn. Neske, PfullingenGoogle Scholar
  7. Blakemore S-J, Decety J (2001) From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nat Rev Neurosci 2:561–567Google Scholar
  8. Blakemore S-J, Oakley DA, Frith CD (2003) Delusions of alien control in the normal brain. Neuropsychologia 41:1058–1067CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Blanke O, Mohr C (2005) Out-of-body experience, heautoscopy, and autoscopic hallucination of neurological origin. Implications for neurocognitive mechanisms of corporeal awareness and self-consciousness. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 50:184–199CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Blanke O, Landis T, Spinelli L, Seeck M (2004) Out-of-body experience and autoscopy of neurological origin. Brain 127:243–258CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Blankenburg W, Mishara AL (trans) (2001) First steps toward a ‘psychopathology of common sense’. Philos Psychiatr Psychol 8:303–315 Originally published in 1969Google Scholar
  12. Bloom P (2004) Descartes’ baby. Basic Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  13. Bloom P, Weisberg DS (2007) Childwood origins of adult resistance to science. Science 316(5827): 996–997Google Scholar
  14. Brandt T, Dieterich M (1999) The vestibular cortex: its locations, functions, and disorders. Ann N Y Acad Sci 871:293–312CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Bruce R (1999) Astral dynamics. A new approach to out-of-body experience. Hampton Roads, Charlottesville, VAGoogle Scholar
  16. Brugger P (2002) Reflective mirrors: perspective-taking in autoscopic phenomena. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 7:179–194CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Brugger P (2007) Hostile interactions between body and self. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 9:210–213Google Scholar
  18. Brugger P, Agosti R, Regard M, Wieser H-G, Landis T (1994) Heautoscopy, epilepsy, and suicide. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 57:838–839CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Brugger P, Regard M, Landis T (1996) Unilaterally felt “presences”: the neuropsychiatry of one’s invisible doppelganger. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 9:114–122Google Scholar
  20. Brugger P, Regard M, Landis T (1997) Illusory reduplication of one’s own body: phenomenology and classification of autoscopic phenomena. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2:19–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Brugger P, Regard M, Landis T, Oelz O (1999) Hallucinatory experiences in extreme-altitude climbers. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 12:67–71Google Scholar
  22. Brugger P, Blanke O, Regard M, Bradford DT, Landis T (2006) Polyopic heautoscopy: case report and review of the literature. Cortex 42:666–674CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Buckner RL, Carroll DC (2007) Self-projection and the brain. Trends Cogn Sci 11:49–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Bünning S, Blanke O (2005) The out-of body experience: precipitating factors and neural correlates. Prog Brain Res 150:331–350CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Cheyne JA (2003) Sleep paralysis and the structure of waking-nightmare hallucinations. Dreaming 13:163–179CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Cheyne JA (2005) Sleep paralysis episode frequency and number, types, and structure of associated hallucinations. J Sleep Res 14:319–324CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Christodoulou GN (1978) The syndrome of subjective doubles. Am J Psychiatry 135:249–251Google Scholar
  28. Claesges U (1964) Edmund Husserls Theorie der Raumkonstitution. Phaenomenologica. Martinus Nijhoff, Den HaagGoogle Scholar
  29. Conrad K (1933) Das Koerperschema. Eine kritische Studie und der Versuch einer Revision. Zeitschrift fiir die gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie 147:346–369CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Conrad K (1953) Über ein eigenartiges Spiegelphantom. Heautoskopisches Phänomen als Dauerzustand bei Hypophysentumor. Nervenarzt 24:265–270Google Scholar
  31. Conrad K (1958) Die beginnende Schizophrenie. Stuttgart, Thieme VerlagGoogle Scholar
  32. Conrad K (1960) Die symptomatischen Psychosen. In: Psychiatrie der Gegenwart, Vol. II, Ed. by H. W. Gruhle, R. Jung, W. Mayer-Gross and W. Mueller, Berlin, Springer, pp. 369–436Google Scholar
  33. Damas Mora JMR, Jenner FA, Eacott SE (1980) On heautoscopy or the phenomenon of the double: case presentation and review of the literature. Br J Med Psychol 53:75–83Google Scholar
  34. De Preester H (2007) The deep bodily origins of the subjective perspective: models and their problems. Conscious Cogn 16(3):604–618CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Decety J, Chaminade T, Grèzes J, Meltzoff AN (2002) A pet exploration of the neural mechanisms involved in reciprocal imitation. Neuroimage 15:265–272CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Denning TR, Berrios GE (1994) Autoscopic phenomena. Br J Psychiatry 165:808–817CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Devinsky O, Feldmann E, Burrowes K, Bromfield E (1989) Autoscopic phenomena with seizures. Arch Neurol 46:1080–1088Google Scholar
  38. Ehrsson HH (2007) The experimental induction of out-of-body experiences. Science 317:1048CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Eichenbaum H, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C (2007) The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annu Rev Neurosci 30:123–152CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Engerth G, Hoff H (1929) Ein Fall von Halluzinationen im hemianoptischen Gesichtsfeld. Beitrag zur Genese der optischen Halluzinationen. Monatsschrift für Psychiatrie und Neurologie 74:246–256CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Feinberg TE (2000) Altered Egos: How the Brain Creates the Self. New York, Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  42. Féré C (1891) Note sur les hallucinations autoscopiques ou spéculaires et sur les hallucinations altruistes. C R Séanc Soc Biol 3:451–453Google Scholar
  43. Foa EB, Riggs DS (1993) Posttraumatic stress disorder and rape. In: Oldham JM, Riba MB, Tasman A (eds) Review of psychiatry, vol 12. American Psychiatric, Washington, DC, pp 273–303Google Scholar
  44. Frith CD (1995) Consciousness is for other people. Behav Brain Sci 18:682–683CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Fuchs T (1995) Coenaesthesie. Zur Geschichte des Gemeingefühls. Z Klin Psychol Psychopathol Psychother 43:103–112Google Scholar
  46. Fuchs T (2006) Ethical issues in neuroscience. Curr Opin Psychiatry 19:600–607CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Fuster JM (2006) The cognit: a network model of cortical representation. Int J Psychophysiol 60:125–132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Gadamer HG (1976) In: Philosophical hermeneutics (trans: Linge DE). University of California Press, BerkeleyGoogle Scholar
  49. Gadamer HG (1993) Truth and method, 2nd revised edition (trans: Weinsheimer J, Masrshall D). Continuum, New York. (Original work published 1960)Google Scholar
  50. Gallagher S (2004) Ways of knowing the self and the other. In Gallagher S, Watson S (eds) Ipseity and alterity: interdisciplinary approaches to intersubjectivity. Publications de l’Université de Rouen, Rouen, pp 7–29Google Scholar
  51. Gallagher S (2005) Metzinger’s matrix: living the virtual life with a real body. Psyche 11:9Google Scholar
  52. Gallagher S, Cole J (1995) Body schema and body image in a deafferented subject. J Mind Behav 16:369–390Google Scholar
  53. Gibson JJ (1958) Visually controlled locomotion and visual orientation in animals. Br J Psychol 1958:182–194Google Scholar
  54. Goethe JW (1790) Versuch die Metamorphose der Pflanzen zu erklären. Carl Wilhelm Ettinger, Gotha; 1984 facsimile. Acta humaniora der Verlag Chemie GmbH, WeinheimGoogle Scholar
  55. Green CE (1968) Out-of-body experiences. Institute of Psychophysical Research, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  56. Grüsser OJ, Landis T (1991) The splitting of “i” and “me”: heautoscopy and related phenomena. In: Grüsser OJ, Landis T (eds) Visual agnosias and other disturbances of visual perception and cognition, vol 12. Macmillan, London, pp 297–303Google Scholar
  57. Grüsser OJ, Pause M, Schreiter U (1990) Localization and responses of neurones in the parieto-insular vestibular cortex of awake monkeys (Macaca fascicularis). J Physiol 430:537–557Google Scholar
  58. Gurwitsch A (1966) Studies in phenomenology and psychology. Northwestern University Press, Evanston, ILGoogle Scholar
  59. Gusnard DA (2005) Being a self: considerations from functional imaging. Conscious Cogn 14:679–697CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Haggard P, Aschersleben G, Gehrke J, Prinz, W (2002) Action, binding and awareness. In B. Hommel, & W. Prinz (Eds.), Attention and Performance XIX: Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action, pp. 266–285. Oxford, England: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  61. Head H (1920) Studies in Neurology, 2. Oxford University Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  62. Head H, Holmes G (1911) Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions. Brain 34:102–254CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Hécaen H, Ajuriaguerra J (1952) L’Heautoscopie, Meconnassiances et Hallucinations L’Héautoscopie. Corporelles, Masson, ParisGoogle Scholar
  64. Heidegger M (1927) Martin Heidegger. Sein und Zeit. Niemeyer, TubingenGoogle Scholar
  65. Held K (2003) Husserl’s phenomenology of the life-world. In: Donn W (ed) The new Husserl: a critical reader. Indiana University Press, Bloomington, pp 32–62Google Scholar
  66. Hoffman RE (2007) A social deafferentation hypothesis for induction of active schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 33:1066–1070CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Hoffman RE, Hawkins KA, Gueorguieva R, Boutros NN, Rachid F, Carroll K, Krystal JH (2003) Transcranial magnetic stimulation of left temporoparietal cortex and medication-resistant auditory hallucinations. Arch Gen Psychiatry 60:49–56CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Hoffmann ETA (1967) The best tales of Hoffmann. Dover, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  69. Hughlings JJ (1932) In: Taylor J (ed) Selected writings of John Hughlings Jackson. London, Hodden and StoughtonGoogle Scholar
  70. Husserl E (1959) Erste Philosophie 1923/24. II. Theorie der Phaenomenologischen Reduktion. Martinus Nijhoff, The HaagGoogle Scholar
  71. Husserl E (1960) Cartesian Meditations: An Introduction to Phenomenology. Cairns, D., trans. The Hague, Martinus Nijhoff. Originally published in 1931Google Scholar
  72. Husserl E (1966) Analysen zur passiven Synthesis aus Forschungsmanuscripten 1918–1926. Martinus Nijhoff, The HagueGoogle Scholar
  73. Husserl E (1973) Experience and Judgment, Investigations in a Geneology of Logic. Evanston, Ill:NorthwesternUniversity PressGoogle Scholar
  74. Husserl E (1988) Cartesian meditations: an introduction to phenomenology. Martinus Nijhoff, The Hague, The Netherlands. Originally published in 1931Google Scholar
  75. Husserl E (1997) Thing and space: lectures of 1907 (trans: Richard R). Kluwer, DordrechtGoogle Scholar
  76. Ichiro Yamaguchi (1982) Passive Synthesis und Intersubjektivtät bei Edmund Husserl. The Hague: Martinus NijhoffGoogle Scholar
  77. Ionasescu V (1960) Paroxysmal disorders of the body image in temporal lobe epilepsy. Acta Psychaitr Scand 35:171–181CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Irwin HJ (1985) Flight of mind: a psychological study of the out-of-body experience. Scarecrow, Metuchen, NJGoogle Scholar
  79. James W (1961) The varieties of religious experience. 1902. Collier Macmillan, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  80. Jantz H, Beringer K (1944) Das syndrom des schwebeerlebnisses unmittelbar nach kopfverletzungen. Nervenarzt 17:197–206Google Scholar
  81. Jaspers K (1913) Über leibhaftige Bewusstheiten. Bewussheitstäuschungen., ein psychopathologisches Elementarsymptom. Z Psychopathol 2:150–161Google Scholar
  82. Jaspers K (1946) Allgemeine psychopathologie. Allgemeine psychopathologie, 4th edition. Springer, Berlin. English edition: General psychopathology (1963) (trans: Hoenig J, Hamilton MW). Manchester University Press, Manchester, UKGoogle Scholar
  83. Kafka F (1965) Diaries (1910–1913) (trans: Joseph K). Schocken Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  84. Kafka F (1979) The basic Kafka. Pocket Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  85. Kamiya S, Okamoto S (1982) Double consciousness in epileptics: a clinical picture and minor hemisphere specialization. In: Akimoto H, Kazamatsuri H, Seino M, Ward A (eds) Advances in epileptology, 13th epilepsy international symposium. Raven, New York, pp 397–401Google Scholar
  86. Kurz G (1980) Traum-Schrecken, Kafkas literarische Existenzanalyse. Stuttgart: J.B. Metzlersche VerlagGoogle Scholar
  87. Lacan J (1977) The mirror stage as formative of the function of the I as revealed in psychoanalytic experience. In: Sheridan A (trans) Écrits: a selection. W. W. Norton, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  88. Legrand D, Newen A, Vogeley K (2003) How not to find the neural signature of self-consciousness. Conscious Cogn 12:544–548CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Lhermitte J (1939) L’Image de Notre Corps. Nouvelle, ParisGoogle Scholar
  90. Lhermitte J (1951) Visual hallucination of the self. Br Med J 1:431–434CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Lhermitte F (1983) ‘Utilization behaviour’ and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes Brain 106:237–255Google Scholar
  92. Lishman JR, Lee DN (1973) The autonomy of visual kinaesthesis. Perception 2:287–294Google Scholar
  93. Lopez C, Halje P, Blanke O (2008) Body ownership and embodiment: vestibular and multisensory mechanisms. Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clin Neurophysiol 38:149–161CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Lukianowicz N (1958) Autoscopic phenomena. Arch Neurol Psychiat 80:199–220Google Scholar
  95. Maravita A, Iriki A (2004) Tools for the body schema. Trends Cogn Sci 8:79–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Menninger-Lerchenthal E (1935) Das Truggebilde der Eigenen Gestall. Karger, BerlinGoogle Scholar
  97. Menninger-Lerchenthal E (1961) Heautoskopie. Wien Med Wochenschr 111:745–756Google Scholar
  98. Merleau-Ponty M (1964) The child’s relation with others. In: Edie JE (ed) The primacy of perception. Northwestern University Press, Evanston, IL, pp 96–155Google Scholar
  99. Merleau-Ponty M (1968) The visible and the invisible (trans: Alphonso L). Northwestern University Press, EvanstonGoogle Scholar
  100. Merleau-Ponty M (1973) In: Claude L (ed) The prose of the world (trans: O’Neill J). Northwestern University Press, EvanstonGoogle Scholar
  101. Metzinger T (2003) Being no one. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  102. Metzinger T (2005) Out-of-body experiences as the origin of the concept of a “soul”. Mind Matter 3:57–84Google Scholar
  103. Metzinger T (2006) Précis: being no one. Psyche 12.4:1–7Google Scholar
  104. Metzinger T (2006b) Reply to Gallagher: different conceptions of embodiment. Psyche 12:1–7Google Scholar
  105. Miall RC (2003) Connecting mirror neurons and forward models. Neuroreport 14:2135–2137Google Scholar
  106. Milner AD, Goodale MA (1995) The visual brain in action. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  107. Mishara AL (1990) Husserl and Freud: time, memory and the unconscious. Husserl Stud 1990(7):29–58CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Mishara AL (1994) A phenomenological critique of commonsensical assumptions of DSM-III-R: the avoidance of the patient’s subjectivity. In: Sadler J, Schwartz M, Wiggins O (eds) Philosophical perspectives on psychiatric diagnostic classification. Johns Hopkins Series in Psychiatry and Neuroscience, Baltimore, pp 129–147Google Scholar
  109. Mishara AL (1995) Narrative and psychotherapy: The phenomenology of healing. American Journal of Psychotherapy 49:180–195Google Scholar
  110. Mishara AL (in press a) The ‘Unconscious’ in Paranoid Delusional Psychosis? Phenomenology, Neuroscience, Psychoanalysis. To appear in: Lohmar, D., Brudzinska, J. (eds.). Founding Psychoanalysis. New York: SpringerGoogle Scholar
  111. Mishara AL (in press b). Klaus Conrad (1905–1961): Why we need to ‘understand’ the patient’s experience to study the neurobiology. Schizophrenia Bulletin Google Scholar
  112. Mishara AL (in press c) Human ambivalence to body: Precondition for Social Cognition and its Disruption in Neuropsychiatric Disorders. Philosophy, Psychiatry & Psychology Google Scholar
  113. Mishara AL (in press d) Kafka’s doubles, paranoia and the brain: Hypnagogic vs. hyper-reflexive models of disruption of self in neuropsychiatric disorders and anomalous conscious states. Philosophy, Ethics, and Humanities in Medicine (PEHM), PubMed Central Open Access Journal http://www.peh-med.com/
  114. Mishara AL (1997) Binswanger and phenomenology. In: Embree L, Behnke EA, Carr D, Evans JC, Huertas-Jourda J, Kockelmans JJ, McKenna W, Mickunas A, Mohanty JN, Seebohm TM, Zaner RM (eds) Encyclopedia of phenomenology. Kluwer, The Hague, pp 62–66Google Scholar
  115. Mishara AL (2001) On Wolfgang Blankenburg, common sense, and schizophrenia. Philos Psychiatry Psychol 8:317–322CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  116. Mishara AL (2004) The disconnection of external and internal in the conscious experience of schizophrenia: phenomenological, literary and neuroanatomical archaeologies of self. Philosophica 73:87–126Google Scholar
  117. Mishara AL (2005) Body self and its narrative representation in schizophrenia: does the body schema concept help establish a core deficit? In: De Preester H, Knockaert V (eds) Body image and body schema. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 127–152Google Scholar
  118. Mishara AL (2007a) Is minimal self preserved in schizophrenia? A subcomponents view. Conscious Cogn 16:715–721CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  119. Mishara AL (2007b) Missing links in phenomenological clinical neuroscience? Why we are still not there yet. Curr Opin Psychiatry 20:559–569CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  120. Mishara AL, Schwartz M (1997) Psychopathology in the light of emergent trends in the philosophy of consciousness, neuropsychiatry and phenomenology. Curr Opin Psychiatry 10:383–389CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  121. Nadler JW, Angelaki DE, DeAngelis GC (2008) A neural representation of depth from motion parallax in macaque visual cortex. Nature 452:642–655CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  122. Navon D (2001) The puzzle of mirror reversal: A view from clockland Psycoloquy 12:1–27Google Scholar
  123. Nowak LG, Bullier J (1997) The timing of information transfer in the visual system. Extrastriate Visual Cortex Primates 12:205–241Google Scholar
  124. Paillard J (1991) Knowing where knowing how to get there. In: Paillard J (ed) Brain and Space. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 461–481Google Scholar
  125. Paillard J (1991a) Knowing where knowing how to get there. In: Paillard J (ed) Brain and Space.Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 461–481Google Scholar
  126. Paillard J (1991b) Motor and representational framing of space. In: Paillard J (1991) Knowing where knowing how to get there. In: Paillard J (ed) Brain and Space. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 163–182Google Scholar
  127. Paillard J (1999) Body schema and body image: d double dissociation in deafferented patients. In: Gantchev GN, Mori S, Massion J (eds) Motor control, today and tomorrow. Academic Publishing House, Sophia, pp 197–214Google Scholar
  128. Paillard J (2005) Vectorial versus configural encoding of body space: a neural basis for a distinction between body schema and body image. In: De Preester H, Knockaert V (eds) Body image and body schema, interdisciplinary perspectives on the body. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 89–109Google Scholar
  129. Plessner H (1975) Die Stufen des Organischen und der Mensch. de Gruyter, Berlin/New YorkGoogle Scholar
  130. Richter JP (1959) Siebenkas. Carl Hansen Verlag, Werke, Munchen (original, 1796)Google Scholar
  131. Ruby P, Decety J (2001) Effect of subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: a PET investigation of agency. Nat Neurosci 4:546–550Google Scholar
  132. Sartre JP (1966) In: Being and nothingness. A phenomenological essay on ontology (trans: Barnes HE). Simon & Schuster, New York (Original, 1943)Google Scholar
  133. Sedman G (1966) A comparative study of pseudohallucinations, imagery and true hallucinations. Br J Psychiatry 112:9–17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  134. Seebohm T (1962) Die Bedingung der Moeglichkeit der Transcendental Philosophie. Bouvier Verlag, BonnGoogle Scholar
  135. Seebohm T (1989) Transcendental phenomenology. In: Mohanty JN, McKenna W (eds) Husserl’s phenomenology: a textbook. CARP and University Press of America, Washington DC, pp 345–385Google Scholar
  136. Silberer H (1909) Bericht über eine Methode, gewisse symbolische Halluzionations-Erscheinungen hervorzurufen und zu beobachten. Jahrbuch für psychoanalytische und psychopathlogische Forschung 3:661–723Google Scholar
  137. Sollier P (1903) Les Phénomènes d’Autoscopie. Félix Alcan, ParisGoogle Scholar
  138. Spence SA, Brooks DJ, Hirsch SR, Liddle PF, Meehan J, Grasby PM (1997) A PET study of voluntary movement in schizophrenic patients experiencing passivity phenomena (delusions of alien control) Brain 120:1997–2011Google Scholar
  139. Stawarska B (2004) The body, the mirror and the other in Merleau-Ponty and Sartre. In: Gallagher S, Watson S (eds) Ipseity and alterity: interdisciplinary approaches to intersubjectivity. Publications de l’Université de Rouen, Rouen, pp 175–186Google Scholar
  140. Stephen JM, Aine CJ, Christner RF, Ranken D, Huang M, Best E (2002) Central versus peripheral visual field stimulation results in timing differences in dorsal stream sources as measured with MEG. Vision Research 42:3059–3074Google Scholar
  141. Tadokoro Y, Oshima T, Kanemoto K (2006) Postictal autoscopy in a patient with partial epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav 9:535–540CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  142. Todd J, Dewhurst K (1955) The double: its psycho-pathology and psychophysiology. J Nerv Ment Dis 122:47–55Google Scholar
  143. Twemlow SW, Gabbard GO, Jones FC (1982) The out-of-body experience: A phenomeno-­logical typology based on questionnaire responses. American Journal of Psychiatry 139:450–455Google Scholar
  144. Uhlhaas PJ, Mishara AL (2007) Perceptual anomalies in schizophrenia: integrating phenomenology and cognitive neuroscience. Schizophr Bull 33:142–156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  145. Vogeley K, Bussfeld P, Newen A, Herrmann S, Happé F, Falkai P, Maier W, Zilles K (2001) Mind reading: neural mechanisms of theory of mind and self-perspective. NeuroImage 14:170–181CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  146. Vogeley K, May M, Ritzl A, Falkai P, Zilles K, Fink GR (2004) Neural correlates of first-person perspective as one constituent of human self-consciousness. J Cogn Neurosci 16:817–827CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  147. Weizsäcker V. von (1997) Der Gestaltkreis, dargestellt als physiologische Analyse des optischen Drehversuchs. Original 1933. In: Viktor von W (ed) Gesammelte Schriften, Bd. 4, (S. 23–61). Suhrkamp, Frankfurt a.M.Google Scholar
  148. Weizsaecker V. von (1950) Der Gestaltkreis. Theorie der Einheit von Wahrnnehmung und Bewegung 4th Aufl. Georg Thieme Verlag, StuttgartGoogle Scholar
  149. Wigan AL (1844) A new view of insanity: the duality of mind. Longman, Brown, Green & Longmans, LondonGoogle Scholar
  150. Wiggins OP (1994) Commentary on self-consciousness, mental agency, and the clinical psychopathology of thought insertion. Philos Psychiatr Psychol 1:11–12Google Scholar
  151. Wiggins OP, Spitzer M (1997) Cognitive science. In: Embree L et al. (ed) Encyclopedia of phenomenology. Kluwer, The Hague, The Netherlands, pp 101–104Google Scholar
  152. Wisdom JO (1953) The concept of ‘‘phantom-body’’. Proceedings of the XIth International Congress of Philosophy: Vol VII: Philosophical psychology (pp. 175–179). Amsterdam: North Holland Publishing CoGoogle Scholar
  153. Wolpert DM, Doya K, Kawato M (2003) A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philos Trans R Soc B: Biol Sci 358:593–602CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  154. Zutt J (1953) Aussersichsein und auf sich selbst zurückblicken als ausnahmezustand. Nervenarzt 24:24–30Google Scholar
  155. Galati G, Committeri G, Sanes JN, Pizzamiglio L (2001) Spatial coding of visual and somatic sensory information in body-centred coordinates. Eur J Neurosci 14.4:737–746CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  • Aaron L. Mishara
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Psychiatry, Clinical Neuroscience Research UnitYale University School of MedicineNew HavenUSA

Personalised recommendations