Advertisement

A Cross-Cultural Analysis of Hunter-Gatherer Social Learning

  • Zachary H. Garfield
  • Melissa J. Garfield
  • Barry S. Hewlett
Part of the Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans Series book series (RNMH)

Abstract

Social learning among hunter-gatherers has been widely discussed in the literature and authors often draw on ethnographic cases to support theoretical models. In this study we report on the cross-cultural occurrence of various modes and processes of social learning in distinct cultural domains from the ethnographic record. To our knowledge this is the first systematic, cross-cultural study of hunter-gatherer social learning. We rely on the sample of hunter-gatherers in the electronic Human Relations Area Files (eHRAF) to generate our source of ethnographic texts. We have coded and analyzed 982 ethnographic texts from 23 diverse societies. Oblique and vertical transmission appear at similar rates. Various forms of teaching are the most common processes of social learning and account for more than half of all coded texts. Vertical and oblique social learning are predominantly characterized by teaching, whereas horizontal social learning is primarily through collaborative learning. Approximations of age reveal a general developmental pattern in which social learning of miscellaneous skills characterizes infancy, subsistence skills dominate early and middle childhood, and the social learning of religious beliefs are most frequent during adolescence. Across development we identify a reduction in the importance of vertical transmission in favor of oblique transmission, for subsistence skills in particular. These results highlight the importance of teaching in the ethnographic record of hunter-gatherer social learning and provide a systematic, cross-cultural, framework for theoretical models to rely on.

Keywords

Hunter-gatherers Human Relations Area Files Social learning Cultural transmission Teaching 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We are grateful for the careful assessment and suggestions from several reviewers. We wish to sincerely thank Victoria Reyes-García, Akira Takada, Wayne Babchuk, and Brett Wertz for their comments and assistance during the preparation of this manuscript.

References

  1. Berl RE, Hewlett BS (2015) Cultural variation in the use of overimitation in the Aka and Ngandu of the Congo basin. PLoS ONE 10:e0120180. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0120180CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bock J (2010) An evolutionary perspective on learning in social, cultural, and ecological context. In: Lancy DF, Bock JF, Gaskins S (eds) The anthropology of learning in childhood. Rowman and Littlefield, Lanham, pp 11–34Google Scholar
  3. Boehm C (1999) Hierarchy in the forest: the evolution of egalitarian behavior. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  4. Boehm C (2008) Purposive social selection and the evolution of human altruism. Cross-Cult Res 42(4):319CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bowlby J (1973) Attachment and loss, vol 2, separation: anxiety and anger. Basic Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  6. Box HO, Gibson KR (1999) Mammalian social learning: comparative and ecological perspectives. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  7. Boyd R, Richerson P (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  8. Brown DE (1991) Human universals. McGraw-Hill, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  9. Burgesse JA (1944) The woman and the child among the Lac-St-Jean Montagnais. Primitive Man 17:1–18Google Scholar
  10. Burkart M, Hrdy S, Van Schaik P (2009) Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evol Anthropol Issues News Rev 18(5):175–186CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Caro TM, Hauser MD (1992) Is there teaching in nonhuman animals? Q Rev Biol 67:151–174Google Scholar
  12. Cavalli-Sforza LL (1981) Cultural transmission and evolution: a quantitative approach. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  13. Cavalli-Sforza LL, Feldman MW, Chen KH, Dornbusch SM (1982) Theory and observation in cultural transmission. Science 218(4567):19–27CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Chudek M, Heller S, Birch S, Henrich J (2011) Prestige-biased cultural learning: bystander’s differential attention to potential models influences children’s learning. Evol Hum Behav 33(1):46–56CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Condon RG (1983) Inuit behavior and seasonal change in the Canadian Arctic. Studies in cultural anthropology. UMI Research Press, Ann ArborGoogle Scholar
  16. Csibra G, Gergely G (2011) Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 366(1567):1149–1157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Cummings V, Jordan P, Zvelebil M (2014) The oxford handbook of the archaeology and anthropology of hunter-gatherers. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dawkins R (2006) The selfish gene. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  19. Dean LG, Kendal RL, Schapiro SJ, Thierry B, Laland KN (2012) Identification of the social and cognitive processes underlying human cumulative culture. Science 335(6072):1114–1118CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Draper P, Cashdan E (1988) Technological change and child behavior among the! Kung. Ethnology 27(4):339–365CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Ember CR (1978) Myths about hunter-gatherers. Ethnology 17(4):439–448CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gergely G, Csibra G (2006) Sylvias recipe: the role of imitation and pedagogy in the transmission of cultural knowledge. Roots Hum Soc Cult Cogn Hum Interact 229–255Google Scholar
  23. Harris PL, Corriveau KH (2011) Young children’s selective trust in informants. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 366(1567):1179–1187CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hauser MD, Chomsky N, Fitch WT (2002) The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 298(5598):1569–1579CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Henrich J, Broesch J (2011) On the nature of cultural transmission networks: evidence from Fijian villages for adaptive learning biases. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 366(1567):1139–1148CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Henrich J, Gil-White FJ (2001) The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evol Hum Behav 22(3):165–196CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hewlett BS, Boyette AH (2012) Play in hunter-gatherers. evolution, early experience and human development: from research to practice and policy 388Google Scholar
  28. Hewlett BS, Cavalli-Sforza LL (1986) Cultural transmission among Aka pygmies. Am Anthropol 88(4):922–934CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hewlett BS, Lamb ME, Leyendecker B, Schölmerich A (2000) Internal working models, trust, and sharing among foragers. Curr Anthropol 41(2):287–297CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hewlett BS, Fouts HN, Boyette AH, Hewlett BL (2011) Social learning among Congo Basin hunter-gatherers. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 366(1567):1168–1178. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0373CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Heyes CM, Ray ED, Mitchell CJ, Nokes T (2000) Stimulus enhancement: controls for social facilitation and local enhancement. Learn Motiv 31(2):83–98CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Hill J (1984) Prestige and reproductive success in man. Ethol Sociobiol 5(2):77–95CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Huntingford GWB (1951) Social institutions of the Dorobo. Anthropos 46:1–48Google Scholar
  34. Kaplan H, Hill K, Lancaster J, Hurtado AM (2000) A theory of human life history evolution: diet intelligence, and longevity. Evol Anthropol 9(4):156–185CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Kelly RL (2013) The foraging spectrum: diversity in hunter-gatherer lifeways. Smithsonian Institute Press, Washington, DCCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Klein M (2015) How to learn about teaching: an evolutionary framework for the study of teaching behavior in humans and other animals. Behav Brain Sci 38(31) doi: 10.1017/S0140525X14000090
  37. Konner M (2010) The evolution of childhood: relationships, emotion, mind. Belknap, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  38. Kruger AC, Tomasello M (1996) Cultural learning and learning culture. In: Olson D, Torrance N (eds) The handbook of education and human development: new models of learning, teaching and schooling. Blackwell Publishing, HobokenGoogle Scholar
  39. Laland KN, Galef BG (2009) The question of animal culture. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  40. Laland KN, Hoppitt W (2003) Do animals have culture? Evol Anthropol Issues News Rev 12(3):150–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Lancy DF (2010) “Learning from nobody”: the limited role of teaching in folk models of children’s development. Child Past 3(1):79–106CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Lancy DF, Grove A (2010) The role of adults in children’s learning. In: Lancy DF, Bock J, Gaskins S (eds) The anthropology of learning in childhood. Altamira Press, PlymouthGoogle Scholar
  43. Lee RB (1979) The !Kung San: men, women, and work in a foraging society. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  44. Lee RB, Daly RH (1999) The cambridge encyclopedia of hunters and gatherers. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  45. Lee RB, DeVore I (1968) Man the hunter. Aldine Publishing, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  46. Lyons DE, Young AG, Keil FC (2007) The hidden structure of overimitation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104(50):19751–19756CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. MacDonald K (2007) Cross-cultural comparison of learning in human hunting. Hum Nat 18(4):386–402CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Maegraith BG (1932) The astronomy of the Aranda and Luritja tribes. Trans R Soc S Aust 56:19–26Google Scholar
  49. Marlowe FW (2005) Hunter-gatherers and human evolution. Evol Anthropol 14(2):54–67CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Marshall L (1957) The kin terminology system of the !Kung bushmen. Africa 27(01):1–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Mead M (1930) Growing up in New Guinea: a comparative study of primitive education. W. Morrow, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  52. Mead M (1964) Continuities in cultural evolution. Transaction Publishers, PiscatawayGoogle Scholar
  53. Munroe RL, Gauvain M (2010) The cross-cultural study of children’s learning and socialization: a short history. In: Lancy DF, Bock J, Gaskins S (eds) The anthropology of learning in childhood. Altamira Press, PlymouthGoogle Scholar
  54. Pagel M (2009) Human language as a culturally transmitted replicator. Nat Rev Genet 10(6):405–415. doi: 10.1038/nrg2560CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Perry S (2011) Social traditions and social learning in capuchin monkeys (cebus). Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 366(1567):988–996CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Pinker S, Jackendoff R (2005) The faculty of language: what’s special about it? Cognition 95(2):201–236CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Reyes-García V, Molina JL, Broesch J, Calvet L, Huanca T, Saus J, Tanner S, Leonard WR, McDade TW, TAPS Bolivian Study Team (2008) Do the aged and knowledgeable men enjoy more prestige? a test of predictions from the prestige-bias model of cultural transmission. Evol Hum Behav 29(4):275–281CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Reyes-García V, Broesch J, Calvet-Mir L, Fuentes-Peláez N, McDade TW, Parsa S, Tanner S, Huanca T, Leonard WR, Martínez-Rodríguez MR (2009) Cultural transmission of ethnobotanical knowledge and skills: an empirical analysis from an Amerindian society. Evol Hum Behav 30:274–285CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Richerson PJ, Boyd R (2005) Not by genes alone: how culture transformed human evolution. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  60. Rogoff B (1990) Apprenticeship in thinking: cognitive development in social context. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  61. Rogoff B, Moore L, Najafi B, Dexter A, Correa-Chávez M, Solis J (2007) Children’s development of cultural repertoires through participation in everyday routines and practices.Google Scholar
  62. Shade CI (1948) Outside man and his relation to Aleut culture. [S.L.]: [s.n.]. Retrieved from http://ehrafworldcultures.yale.edu/document?id=na06-020
  63. Slocum S (1975) Woman the gatherer: male bias in anthropology. In: Reiter RR (ed) Toward an anthropology of women. Monthly Review Press, New York, pp 36–50Google Scholar
  64. Thornton A, Raihani NJ (2008) The evolution of teaching. Anim Behav 75(6):1823–1836CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Tomasello M (2009) The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  66. Tomasello M, Kruger AC, Ratner HH (1993) Cultural learning. Behav Brain Sci 16(03):495–511CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Tooby J, Cosmides L (1990) The past explains the present: emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethol Sociobiol 11(4):375–424CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Turnbull CM (1983) The Mbuti pygmies: change and adaptation. Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, AustinGoogle Scholar
  69. Tylor EB (1871) Primitive culture: researches into the development of mythology, philosophy, religion, art, and custom, vol 2. Murray, LondonGoogle Scholar
  70. VanStone JW (1963) The snowdrift Chipewyan, vol 63. Northern Co-ordination and Research Centre, Department of Northern Affairs and National Resources.Google Scholar
  71. Whiten A (2011) The scope of culture in chimpanzees, humans and ancestral apes. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 366(1567):997–1007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Wiessner PW (1978) Hxaro: a regional system of reciprocity for reducing risk among the !Kung San, vol 1. University of Michigan, Ann ArborGoogle Scholar
  73. Zarger RK (2010) Learning the environment. In: Lancy DF, Bock J, Gaskins S (eds) The anthropology of learning in childhood. Altamira Press, Plymouth, p 341Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Japan 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Zachary H. Garfield
    • 1
  • Melissa J. Garfield
    • 1
  • Barry S. Hewlett
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of AnthropologyWashington State UniversityVancouverUSA

Personalised recommendations