Advertisement

Children’s Play and the Integration of Social and Individual Learning: A Cultural Niche Construction Perspective

  • Adam Howell Boyette
Chapter
Part of the Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans Series book series (RNMH)

Abstract

In this chapter, I explore the integration of imitative and creative aspects of children’s autonomous play as a means of examining the evolved psychology for learning within a culturally constructed niche. I investigate whether children’s play tracks expected pathways of cultural transmission across a small sample of foraging and agrarian societies, and what the settings of Aka forager and Ngandu farmer children’s play reveal about the daily lived experiences that lead to culture learning—especially the role of collaborative learning across middle childhood. My cross-cultural analysis supports the hypothesis that social stratification is associated with horizontal social learning as represented by the frequency children play games. Additionally, however, a high percentage of play across the cultures analyzed represented spontaneous, creative play, indicative of a strong and universal preference for individual learning, which is conducive to innovation. My analysis of the settings of Aka forager and Ngandu farmer children’s play problematizes the idea that vertical and oblique cultural transmission is in practice the movement of information from older to younger individuals. In fact, children’s imitation of traditional (i.e., learned from adults) activities in play often occurred away from adults. These results are discussed in light of previous studies of play and cultural transmission.

Keywords

Play Learning Cultural niche construction Cultural transmission Foragers 

References

  1. Aunger R (2000) The life history of culture learning in a face-to-face society. Ethos 28(3):445–481CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Aunger R (2002) Exposure versus susceptibility in the epidemiology of “everyday” beliefs. J Cogn Cult 2(2):113–154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bahuchet S, Guillaume H (1982) Aka-farmer relations in the northwest Congo Basin. In: Leacock EB, Lee RB (eds) Politics and history in band societies (trans: van Wyck SM). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 189–212Google Scholar
  4. Bateson G (1972) Steps to an ecology of mind; collected essays in anthropology, psychiatry, evolution, and epistemology. Chandler Publishing, San FranciscoGoogle Scholar
  5. Bird-David N (1990) The giving environment: another perspective on the economic system of gatherer-hunters. Curr Anthropol 31(2):189–196CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Blatchford P, Baines E, Pellegrini AD (2003) The social context of school playground games: sex and ethnic differences, and changes over time after entry to junior school. Br J Dev Psychol 21(481–505):481–505CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Blurton Jones N, Hawkes K, Draper P (1994) Foraging returns of !Kung adults and children: why didn’t !Kung children forage? J Anthropol Res 50(3):217–246CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bock J, Johnson SE (2004) Subsistence ecology and play among the Okavango Delta peoples of Botswana. Hum Nat 15(1):63–81CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bogin B (1990) The evolution of human childhood. Bioscience 40(1):16–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bourdieu P (1977) Outline of a theory of practice. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  12. Boyd R, Richerson PJ, Henrich J (2011) Colloquium paper: the cultural niche: why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proc Natl Acad Sci 108(Suppl 2):10918–10925CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Boyette AH (2013) Social learning during middle childhood among Aka forest foragers and Ngandu farmers of the Central African Republic. Dissertation. Washington State UniversityGoogle Scholar
  14. Boyette AH (2016) Children’s play and culture learning in an egalitarian forager society. Child Dev 87(3):759–769CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Byers JA (1998) The biology of human play. Child Dev 69(3):599–600CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Byers JA, Walker C (1995) Refining the motor training hypothesis for the evolution of play. Am Nat 146(1):25–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Cavalli-Sforza LL, Feldman MW (1981) Cultural transmission and evolution: a quantitative approach. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  18. Demps K, Zorondo-Rodríguez F, García C, Reyes-García V (2012) Social learning across the life cycle: cultural knowledge acquisition for honey collection among the Jenu Kuruba, India. Evol Hum Behav 33:460–470CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Draper P, Cashdan EA (1988) Technological change and child behavior among the !Kung. Ethnology 27(4):339–365CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Enquist M, Eriksson K, Ghirlanda S (2007) Critical social learning: a solution to Rogers’s paradox of nonadaptive culture. Am Anthropol 109(4):727–734CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Giddens A (1984) The constitution of society: outline of the theory of structuration. University of California Press, BerkeleyGoogle Scholar
  22. Gosso Y, Otta E, de Lima M, Morais SE, Ribeiro FJL, Bussab VSR (2005) Play in hunter-gatherer society. In: Pellegrini AD, Smith PK (eds) The nature of play: great apes and humans. Guilford Press, New York, pp 213–253Google Scholar
  23. Harris JR (1995) Where is the child’s environment? A group socialization theory of development. Psychol Rev 102(3):458–489CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hattori S (n.d.) “My medicine (ma a le)”: variability of medicinal plant knowledge among adult Baka hunter-gatherers of Southeast Cameroon. Unpublished ReportGoogle Scholar
  25. Henrich J, Broesch J (2011) On the nature of cultural transmission networks: evidence from Fijian villages for adaptive learning biases. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 366:1139–1148CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Henrich J, Henrich N (2010) The evolution of cultural adaptations: Fijian food taboos protect against dangerous marine toxins. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 277:3715–3724CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hewlett BS (1991) Intimate fathers: the nature and context of Aka pygmy paternal infant care. University of Michigan Press, Ann ArborCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hewlett BL (2013) “Ekeloko”: the spirit to create-Innovation and social learning among Aka adolescents of the Central African Rainforest. In: Akazawa T, Nishiaki Y, Aoki K (eds) Dynamics of learning in Neanderthals and modern humans, vol 1. Springer, Japan, pp 187–195CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hewlett BS, Boyette AH (2012) Play in hunter-gatherers (commentary). In: Narvaez D (ed) Evolution, early experience and human development: from research to practice to policy. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 388–396CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hewlett BS, Cavalli-Sforza LL (1986) Cultural transmission among Aka Pygmies. Am Anthropol 88(4):922–934CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hewlett BS, Fouts HN, Boyette AH, Hewlett BL (2011) Social learning among Congo Basin hunter-gatherers. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 366:1168–1178CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kamei N (2005) Play among Baka children in Cameroon. In: Hewlett BS, Lamb ME (eds) Hunter-gatherer childhoods: evolutionary, developmental, and cultural perspectives. Aldine Transaction, New Brunswick, pp 343–362Google Scholar
  33. Konner M (1972) Aspects of the developmental ethology of a foraging people. In: Blurton Jones N (ed) Ethological studies of child behavior. Cambridge University Press, London, pp 285–304Google Scholar
  34. Konner M (2005) Hunter-gatherer infancy and childhood: the !Kung and others. In: Hewlett BS, Lamb ME (eds) Hunter-gatherer childhoods: evolutionary, developmental, and cultural perspectives. Aldine Transaction, New Brunswick, pp 19–64Google Scholar
  35. Lancy DF (1980) Becoming a blacksmith in Gbarngasuakwelle. Anthropol Educ Q 11(4):266–274CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Lancy DF (2008) The anthropology of childhood: cherubs, chattel, changelings. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  37. Lancy DF (2012) Ethnographic perspectives on cultural transmission/acquisition. Paper prepared for the School of American Research, Santa Fe, Advanced Seminar: Multiple perspectives on the evolution of childhoodGoogle Scholar
  38. Lee RB, Daly R (1999) Foragers and others. In: Lee RB, Daly R (eds) Cambridge encyclopedia of hunters and gatherers. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 1–7Google Scholar
  39. Lewis J (2002) Forest hunter-gatherers and their world: a study of the Mbendjele Yaka Pygmies of Congo-Brazzaville and their secular and religious activities and representations. Doctoral Thesis. London School of Economics and Political ScienceGoogle Scholar
  40. Lewis J (2009) As well as words: Congo Pygmy hunting, mimicry, and play. In: Botha R, Knight C (eds) The cradle of language. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 236–256Google Scholar
  41. Maital SL, Bornstein MH (2003) The ecology of collaborative child rearing: a systems approach to child care on the Kibbutz. Ethos 31(2):274–306CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Martin P, Caro TM (1985) On the function of play and its role in behavioral development. In: Rosenblatt J (ed) Advances in the study of behavior, vol 15. Academic, Orlando, pp 59–103CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. McElreath R, Strimling P (2008) When natural selection favors imitation of parents. Curr Anthropol 49(2):307–316CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Morelli G, Rogoff B, Angelillo C (2003) Cultural variation in young children’s access to work or involvement in specialised child-focused activities. Int J Behav Dev 27:264–274Google Scholar
  45. Ochs E (1988) Culture and language development: language acquisition and language socialization in a Samoan village. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  46. Odling-Smee FJ, Laland KN, Feldman MW (2003) Niche construction: the neglected process in evolution. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  47. Ohmagari K, Berkes F (1997) Transmission of indigenous knowledge and bush skills among the Western James Bay Cree women of Subarctic Canada. Hum Ecol 25(2):197–222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Paradise R, Rogoff B (2009) Side by side: learning by observing and pitching in. Ethos 37(1):102–138CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Pellegrini AD, Bjorklund DF (2004) The ontogeny and phylogeny of children’s object and fantasy play. Hum Nat 15(1):23–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Pellegrini AD, Pellegrini AFA (2012) Play, plasticity, and ontogeny in childhood. In: Narvaez D, Panksepp J, Schore AN, Gleason TR (eds) Evolution, early experience and human development: from research to practice. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 339–351CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Pellegrini AD, Smith PK (1998a) Physical activity play: the nature and function of a neglected aspect of play. Child Dev 69:577–598CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Pellegrini AD, Smith PK (1998b) The development of play during childhood: forms and possible functions. Child Psychol Psychiatry Rev 3(2):51–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Piaget J (1932) The moral judgment of the child (trans: Gabain M). Free Press, GlencoeGoogle Scholar
  54. Reyes-Garcia V, Broesch J, Calvet-Mir L, Fuentes-Peláez N, McDade TW, Parsa S, Tanner S, Huanca T, Leonard WR, Martínez-Rodríguez MR (2009) Cultural transmission of ethnobotanical knowledge and skills: an empirical analysis from an Amerindian society. Evol Hum Behav 30:274–285CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Roberts JM, Sutton-Smith B (1962) Child training and game involvement. Ethnology 1(2):166–185CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Roberts JM, Arth MJ, Bush RR (1959) Games in culture. Am Antiq 61:597–605Google Scholar
  57. Rogoff B, Sellers MJ, Pirrotta S, Fox N, White SH (1975) Age of assignment of roles and responsibilities to children: a cross-cultural survey. Hum Dev 18:353–369CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Roopnarine JL, Johnson JE (1994) The need to look at play in diverse cultural settings. In: Children’s play in diverse cultures. State University of New York Press, Albany, pp 1–8Google Scholar
  59. Smith PK (1978) A longitudinal study of social participation in preschool children: solitary and parallel play reexamined. Dev Psychol 14(5):517–523CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Tehrani JJ, Collard M (2009) On the relationship between interindividual cultural transmission and population-level cultural diversity: a case study of weaving in Iranian tribal populations. Evol Hum Behav 30:286–300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Tomasello M, Kruger AC, Ratner HH (1993) Culture learning. Behav Brain Sci 16:495–552CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Vygotsky LS (1978) Mind in society: the development of higher psychological processes. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  63. Wiessner P (2005) Norm enforcement among the Ju/’hoansi Bushmen. Hum Nat 16(2):115–145CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Japan 2016

Open Access This chapter is licensed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 2.5 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/2.5/), which permits any noncommercial use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license and indicate if changes were made.

The images or other third party material in this chapter are included in the chapter's Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the chapter's Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder.

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Trinity College of Arts & SciencesDuke UniversityDurhamUSA

Personalised recommendations