Lernen, Motivation und Emotion pp 177-187 | Cite as
Wozu haben wir Emotionen?
Zusammenfassung
Wie öde wäre ein Leben ohne Emotionen, ohne das überwältigende Gefühl der Freude bei der Geburt des eigenen Kindes oder den Schmerz beim Verlust einer geliebten Person. Andere Ereignisse lassen uns dagegen kalt, wir empfinden keine Regung. Offensichtlich sagen uns Emotionen etwas über die Bedeutsamkeit von Ereignissen. Sie verändern dabei gleichzeitig die Art und Weise, wie wir die Umwelt wahrnehmen. Wir kennen das alle: Sind wir traurig, erscheint uns die Welt grau und wir kommen nur schlecht auf positive Gedanken. Sind wir dagegen glücklich, dann sieht die Welt ganz anders aus, unsere Gedanken und Gefühle sprudeln nur so aus uns heraus. Irgendwie begleiten uns Emotionen permanent, in dem was wir tun und erleben. Welche Funktion haben sie?
Literatur
- Anderson, A. K. (2005). Affective influences on the attentional dynamics supporting awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(2), 258–281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bless, H., Bohner, G., Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1990). Mood and persuasion a cognitive response analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16(2), 331–345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36(2), 129–148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bower, G. H. (1992). How might emotions affect learning? In S. Christianson (Hrsg.), The handbook of emotion and memory: Research and theory (S. 3–31). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
- Christianson, S.-A. (1992). The handbook of emotion and memory: Research and theory. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
- Donovan, R. J., Rossiter, J. R., Marcoolyn, G., & Nesdale, A. (1994). Store atmosphere and purchasing behavior. Journal of Retailing, 70(3), 283–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). The repertoire of nonverbal behavior: Categories, origins, usage, and coding. Semiotica, 1(1), 49–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2008). Social functions of emotion. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Hrsg.), Handbook of emotions (S. 456–468). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
- Forgas, J. P., & Bower, G. H. (1987). Mood effects on person-perception judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(1), 53–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J., & Rapson, R. (1992). Primitive emotional contagion. In M. Clark (Hrsg.), Review of personality and social psychology: Emotion and social behavior (Bd. 14, S. 151–177). Newbury Park: Sage.Google Scholar
- Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional contagion. Cambridge: University Press.Google Scholar
- Hoffmann, M. L. (1982). Development of prosocial motivation: Empathy and guilt. In N. Eisenberg (Hrsg.), The development of prosocial behavior (S. 281–313). New York: Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Holodynski, M., & Friedlmeier, W. (2006). Development of emotions and emotion regulation. Springer Science & Business Media, New York.Google Scholar
- Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 1122–1131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (1999). Social functions of emotions at four levels of analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 13(5), 505–521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- LaBar, K. S., & Cabeza, R. (2006). Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(1), 54–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lazarus, R. S. (1966). Psychological stress and the coping process. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
- Lien, N.-H. (2001). Elaboration likelihood model in consumer research: A review. Proceedings of the National Science Council Part C: Humanities and Social Sciences, 11(4), 301–310.Google Scholar
- Mandler, G. (1964). The interruption of behavior. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 12, 163–219.Google Scholar
- Matsumoto, D. (2006). Culture and nonverbal behavior. In V. Manusov & M. L. Patterson (Hrsg.), The Sage handbook of nonverbal communication (S. 219–235). Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- McGaugh, J. L. (2004). The Amygdala Modulates the Consolidation of Memories of Emotionally Arousing Experiences. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27(1), 1–28.Google Scholar
- Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Hrsg.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Bd. 19, S. 123–205). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
- Plutchik, R. (2001). The nature of emotions: Human emotions have deep evolutionary roots, a fact that may explain their complexity and provide tools for clinical practice. American Scientist, 89(4), 344–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(3), 380–391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Rucker, D. D., & Petty, R. E. (2004). Emotion specificity and consumer behavior: Anger, sadness, and preference for activity. Motivation and Emotion, 28(1), 3–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Safer, M. A., Christianson, S.-Å., Autry, M. W., & Österlund, K. (1998). Tunnel memory for traumatic events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12(2), 99–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Scherer, K. R. (1982). Emotion as a process: Function, origin and regulation. Social Science Information/sur les sciences sociales, 21(4–5), 555–570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Schimmack, U. (2005). Attentional interference effects of emotional pictures: Threat, negativity, or arousal? Emotion, 5(1), 55–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Schwarz, N. (2012). Feelings-as-information theory. In P. A. M. Van Lange, A. Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Hrsg.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (S. 289–308). Thousand Oaks: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Strauss, G. P., & Allen, D. N. (2006). The experience of positive emotion is associated with the automatic processing of positive emotional words. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 1(3), 150–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Stroop, J. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120(1), 3–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition and Emotion, 24(1), 3–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar