Advertisement

Executive frontal functions

  • Joaquín M. Fuster
Chapter

Abstract

This chapter presents a conceptual model of the representational and executive functions of the cortex of the frontal lobe derived from empirical evidence obtained principally in the monkey. According to this model, the neuronal networks of the frontal lobe that represent motor or executive memories are probably the same networks that cooperate with other cerebral structures in the temporal organization of behavior. The prefrontal cortex, at the top of the perception-action cycle, plays a critical role in the mediation of contingencies of action across time, an essential aspect of the temporal organization of behavior. That role of cross-temporal mediation is based on the interplay of two short-term cognitive functions: one retrospective, of short-term memory or sensory working memory, and the other prospective, of attentive set (or motor working memory). Both appear represented in the neuronal populations of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. At least one of the mechanisms for the retention of working memory of either kind seems to be the reentry of excitability through recurrent cortical circuits. With those two complementary and temporally symmetrical cognitive functions of active memory for the sensory past and for the motor future, the prefrontal cortex secures the temporal closure at the top of the perception-action cycle.

Key words

Prefrontal cortex Cross-temporal contingencies Short-term memory Motor attention Perception-action cycle 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Aguirre GK, Zarahn E, D’Esposito M (1998) Neural components of topographical representation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:839–846PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Courtney SM, Ungerleider LG, Keil K, Haxby JV (1996) Object and spatial visual working memory activate separate neural systems in human cortex. Cereb Cortex 6:39–49PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Funahashi S, Bruce CJ, Goldman-Rakic PS (1989) Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey’s dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J Neurophysiology 61:331–349Google Scholar
  4. Fuster JM (1973) Unit activity in prefrontal cortex during delayed-response performance: neuronal correlates of transient memory. J Neurophysiol 36:61–78PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Fuster JM (1997a) The prefrontal cortex anatomy: physiology and neuropsychology of the frontal lobe. Lippincott-Raven, PhiladelphiaGoogle Scholar
  6. Fuster JM (1997b) Network memory. Trends Neurosci 20:451–459PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Fuster JM, Bauer RH, Jervey JP (1982) Cellular discharge in the dorsolateral pefrontal cortes of the monkey in cognitive tasks. Exp Neurol 77:679–694PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Fuster JM, Bauer RH, Jervey JP (1985) Functional interactions between inferotemporal and prefrontal cortex in a cognitive task. Brain Res 330:299–307PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Gabrieli JDE, Poldrack RA, Desmond JE (1998) The role of left prefrontal cortex in language and memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:906–913PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Goldman-Rakic PS (1995) Architecture of the prefrontal and the central executive. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 769:71–83Google Scholar
  11. Grafton ST, Mazziotta JC, Woods RP, Phelps ME (1992) Human functional anatomy of visually guided finger movements. Brain 115:565–587PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Ingvar DH (1985) “Memory of the future”: an essay on the temporal organization of conscious awareness. Human Neurobiol 4:127–136Google Scholar
  13. Niki H (1974) Differential activity of prefrontal units during right and left delayed response trials. Brain Res 70:346–349PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Owen AM, Evans AC, Petrides M (1996) Evidence for a two-stage model of spatial working memory processing within the lateral frontal cortex: a positron emission tomography study. Cereb Cortex 6:31–38PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Paulesu E, Frith CD, Frackowiak RSJ (1993) The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory. Nature 362: 342–344PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Petrides M, Pandya DN (1994) Comparative architectonic analysis of the human and the macaque frontal cortex. In: Boller F, Grafman J (eds) Handbook of neuropsychology. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 17–58Google Scholar
  17. Petrides M, Alivisatos B, Evans AC, Meyer E (1993a) Dissociation of human mid-dorsolateral from posterior dorsolateral frontal cortex in memory processing. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 90:873–877PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Petrides M, Alivisatos B, Meyer E, Evans AC (1993b) Functional activation of the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory tasks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 90:878–882PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Quintana J, Fuster J (1999) From perception to action: temporal integrative functions of prefrontal and parietal neurons. Cereb Cortex 9:213–221PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Raichle ME, Fiez JA, Videen TO, MacLeod A-MK, Pardo JV, Fox PT, Petersen SE (1994) Practice-related changes in human brain functional anatomy during nonmotor learning. Cereb Cortex 4:8–26PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Smith EE, Jonides J, Koeppe RA (1996) Dissociating verbal and spatial working memory using PET. Cereb Cortex 6:11–20PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Swartz BE, Halgren E, Fuster JM, Simpkins F, Gee M, Mandelkern M (1995) Cortical metabolic activation in humans during a visual memory task. Cereb Cortex 3:205–214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Zipser D, Kehoe B, Littlewort G, Fuster J (1993) A spiking network model of short-term active memory. J Neurosci 13:3406–3420PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2000

Authors and Affiliations

  • Joaquín M. Fuster
    • 1
  1. 1.UCLA Neuropsychiatrie InstituteLos AngelesUSA

Personalised recommendations