Advertisement

Funktionell-neuroanatomische Grundlagen der Theory of Mind

  • Monika SommerEmail author
  • Katrin Döhnel
  • Tobias Schuwerk
  • Göran Hajak
Chapter

Zusammenfassung

Die Fähigkeit, sich und anderen mentale Zustände zuschreiben zu können, stellt die Voraussetzung für ein erfolgreiches Navigieren durch die soziale Welt dar. Um das Verhalten anderer richtig vorherzusagen und zu erklären, müssen wir verstehen, dass andere Personen mentale Zustände wie Wünsche und Überzeugungen haben, nach denen sie handeln.

Literatur

  1. Abraham A, Rakoczy H, Werning M et al (2010) Matching mind to world and vice versa: functional dissociations between belief and desire mental state processing. Social Neurosci 5: 1–18CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Aichhorn M, Perner J, Weiss B et al (2009) Temporo-parietal junction activity in theory-of-mind tasks: falseness, beliefs, or attention. J Cogn Neurosci 21: 1179–1192PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Amodio DM, Frith CD (2006) Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 7: 268–277PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bird CM, Castelli F, Malik O et al (2004) The impact of extensive medial frontal lobe damage on ’Theory of Mind’ and cognition. Brain 127: 914–928PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Blakemore SJ (2008) Development of the social brain during adolescence. Quart J Exp Psychol 61: 40–49CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Blakemore SJ, den Ouden HEM, Choudhury S, Frith C (2007) Adolescent development of the neural circuitry for thinking about intentions. SCAN 2: 130–139PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Brunet E, Sarfati Y, Hardy-Bayle MC, Decety J (2000) A PET investigation of the attribution of intentions with a nonverbal task. NeuroImage 11: 157–166PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Burnett S, Bird G, Moll J et al (2009) Development during adolescence of the neural processing of social emotion. J Cogn Neurosci 21: 1736–1750PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Carrington SJ, Bailey AJ (2009) Are there theory of mind regions in the brain? A review of the neuroimaging literature. Hum Brain Mapping 30: 2313–2335CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL (2008) The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to Theory of Mind. Neuron 58: 306–324PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Decety J, Lamm C (2007) The role of the right temporoparietal junction in social interaction: how low-level computational processes contribute to meta-cognition. Neuroscientist 13: 580–593PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Frith CD, Frith U (2003) Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Phil Trans R Soc B: Biol Sci 358: 459–473CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Frith CD, Frith U. (2006) The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron 50: 531–534PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gallagher HL, Frith CD (2003) Functional imaging of ‚theory of mind‘. Trends Cogn Sci 7: 77–83PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gallagher HL, Happè F, Brunswick N et al (2000) Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‚theory of mind‘ in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia 38: 11–21PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gallagher HL, Jack AI, Roepstorff A, Frith CD (2002) Imaging the intentional stance in a competitive game. NeuroImage 16: 814–821PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gerrans P, Stone VE (2008) Generous or parsimonious cognitive architecture? Cognitive neuroscience and Theory of Mind. Br J Phil Sci 59: 121–141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gobbini MI, Koralek AC, Bryan RE et al (2007) Two takes on the social brain: a comparison of Theory of Mind tasks. J Cogn Neurosci 19: 1803–1814PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gogtay N, Giedd JN, Lusk L et al (2004) Dynamic mapping of the human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101: 8174–8179PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Huttenlocher PR, Dabholkar AS (1997) Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. J Comp Neurol 387: 167–178PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Johnson MH, Grossmann T, Cohen Kadosh K (2009) Mapping functional brain development: building a social brain through interactive specialization. Dev Psychol 45: 151–159PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kobayashi C, Glover GH, Temple E (2007) Children’s and adults’ neural bases of verbal and nonverbal `theory of mind’. Neuropsychologia 45: 1522–1532PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. McGivern RF, Andersen J, Byrd D et al (2002) Cognitive efficiency on a match to sample task decreases at the onset of puberty in children. Brain Cogn: 50: 73–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Meinhardt J, Sodian B, Thoermer C et al (2011) True- and false-belief reasoning in children and adults: an event-related potential study of theory of mind. Dev Cog Neurosci 1: 67–76CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Mitchell JP (2008) Activity in right temporo-parietal junction is not selective for Theory-of-Mind. Cerebr Cortex 18: 262–227CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Mitchell JP, Macrae CN, Banaji MR (2006) Dissociable medial prefrontal contributions to judgments of similar and dissimilar others. Neuron 50(4): 655–663PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Monk CS, McClure EB, Nelson EE et al (2003) Adolescent immaturity in attention-related brain engagement to emotional facial expressions. NeuroImage 20: 420–428PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Pfeifer JH, Lieberman MD, Dapretto M (2007) I know you are but what am I?!: neural bases of self- and social knowledge retrieval in children and adults. J Cogn Neurosci 19: 1323–1337PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Rothmayr C, Sodian B, Hajak G et al (2011) Common and distinct neural networks for false-belief reasoning and inhibitory control. NeuroImage 56(3): 1705–1713PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Saxe R (2010) The right temporo-parietal junction: a specific brain region for thinking about thoughts. In: Leslie A, German T (eds) Handbook of Theory of Mind. Psychology Press, New York, in pressGoogle Scholar
  31. Saxe R, Powell LJ (2006) It’s the thought that counts: specific brain regions for one component of theory of mind. Psychol Sci 17: 692–699PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Saxe R, Carey S, Kanwisher N (2004) Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. Ann Rev Psychol 55: 87–124CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Scholz J, Triantafyllou C, Whitfield-Gabrieli S et al (2009) Distinct regions of right temporo-parietal junction are selective for Theory of Mind and exogenous attention. PLoS ONE 4: e4869PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Shaw P, Kabani NJ, Lerch JP et al (2008) Neurodevelopmental trajectories of the human cerebral cortex. J Neurosci 28: 3586–3594PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Sommer M, Döhnel K, Sodian B et al (2007) Neural correlates of true and false belief reasoning. NeuroImage 35: 1378–1384PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Sommer M, Sodian B, Döhnel K et al (2010a) In psychopathic patients emotion attribution modulates activity in outcome-related brain areas. Psychiatry Res: Neuroimaging 182: 88–95CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Sommer M, Meinhardt J, Eichenmüller K et al (2010b) Modulation of the cortical network associated with false belief reasoning during development. Brain Res 1345: 123–131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Leonard CM et al (2004) Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. J Neurosci 24: 8223–8231PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Tavares P, Lawrence AD, Barnard PJ (2008) Paying attention to social meaning: an fMRI study. Cerebr Cortex 18: 1876–1885CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Toga AW, Thompson PM, Sowell ER (2006) Mapping brain maturation. Trends Neurosci 29: 148–159PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Van Overwalle F (2009) Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. Hum Brain Mapping 30: 829–858CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Young L, Cushman F, Hauser M, Saxe R (2007) The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgement. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104: 8235–8240PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Yurgelun-Todd DA, Killgore WDS (2006) Fear-related activity in the prefrontal cortex increases with age during adolescence: a preliminary fMRI study. Neurosci Lett 406: 194–199PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Wang AT, Lee SS, Sigman M, Dapretto M (2006) Developmental changes in the neural basis of interpreting communicative intent. Social Cogn Affect Neurosci 1: 107–121CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • Monika Sommer
    • 1
    Email author
  • Katrin Döhnel
    • 1
  • Tobias Schuwerk
    • 2
  • Göran Hajak
    • 3
  1. 1.Klinik für Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie derUniversität Regensburg am BezirksklinikumRegensburgDeutschland
  2. 2.Lehrstuhl für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische PsychologieLudwig-Maximilians-Universität MünchenMünchenDeutschland
  3. 3.Klinik für Psychiatrie, Psychosomatik und PsychotherapieSozialstiftung BambergBambergDeutschland

Personalised recommendations