Advertisement

Evolutionarily Stable Communication and Pragmatics

  • Thomas C. Scott-Phillips
Part of the Lecture Notes in Computer Science book series (LNCS, volume 6207)

Abstract

In the past 20 or so years there has been much research interest in the evolution of cooperation in humans (Axelrod, 1995; Boyd & Richerson, 1992; Fehr & Fischbacher, 2003; Milinski et al., 2002; West et al., 2006). The foundational problem addressed by this work is how cooperation can remain evolutionarily stable when individuals have incentives to freeride; that is, to take but not contribute from the public good (Hardin, 1968). There is an analogous problem associated with the evolution of communication: how can signalling remain evolutionarily stable when individuals have incentives to be dishonest? This gametheoretic question is the defining problem of animal signalling theory (Maynard Smith & Harper, 2003; Searcy & Nowicki, 2007). The main goals of this chapter are to explore the various possible solutions to this problem and to ask which most likely applies to human communication. In addition to this it will also, using insights from pragmatics, provide some insight as to the nature of the problem and hence clarify some of the relevant issues.

Keywords

Signal Form Evolutionary Stability Theoretical Biology Stable Communication Repeated Interaction 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Anolli, L., Ciceri, R.: The voice of deception: Vocal strategies of naive and able liars. Journal of Nonverbal Behaviour 21, 259–284 (1997)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Axelrod, R.: The Evolution of Cooperation. Basic Books, New York (1995)zbMATHGoogle Scholar
  3. Axelrod, R., Hamilton, W.D.: The evolution of cooperation. Science 211, 1390–1396 (1981)MathSciNetCrossRefzbMATHGoogle Scholar
  4. Bateson, M., Nettle, D., Roberts, G.: Cues of being watched enhance cooperation in a real-world setting. Biology Letters, 412–414 (2007)Google Scholar
  5. Bond Jr., C.F., Omar, A., Mahmoud, A., Bonser, R.N.: Lie detection across cultures. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 14(3), 189–204 (1990)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Boyd, R., Richerson, P.J.: Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizable groups. Ethology and Sociobiology 13, 171–195 (1992)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cheney, D.L., Seyfarth, R.M.: How Monkeys see the World. University of Chicago Press, Chicago (1990)Google Scholar
  8. Chiappe, D., Brown, D., Dow, B., Koontz, J., Rodreiguez, M., McCulloch, K.: Cheaters are looked at longer and remembered better than cooperators in social exchange situations. Evolutionary Psychology 2, 108–120 (2004)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Clutton-Brock, T.H., Parker, G.A.: Punishment in animal societies. Nature 373, 209–216 (1995)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dessalles, J.-L.: Altruism, status and the origin of relevance. In: Hurford, J.R., Studdert-Kennedy, M., Knight, C. (eds.) Approaches to the Evolution of Language, pp. 130–147. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1998)Google Scholar
  11. Dessalles, J.-L.: Why We Talk: The Evolutionary Origins of Language. Oxford University Press, Oxford (2007)Google Scholar
  12. Dunbar, R.I.M.: Grooming, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language. Faber, London (1997)Google Scholar
  13. Ekman, P.: An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion 6, 169–200 (1992)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Enquist, M., Leimar, O.: The evolution of cooperation in mobile organisms. Animal Behaviour 45(4), 747–757 (1993)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Fehr, E.: Don’t lose your reputation. Nature 432, 449–450 (2004)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Fehr, E., Fischbacher, U.: The nature of human altruism. Nature 425, 785–791 (2003)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Fitch, W.T.: Evolving honest communication systems: Kin selection and mother tongues. In: Oller, D.K., Griebel, U. (eds.) The Evolution of Communication Systems: A Comparative Approach, pp. 275–296. MIT Press, Cambridge (2004)Google Scholar
  18. Fitch, W.T., Reby, D.: The descended larynx is not uniquely human. In: Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, series B, vol. 268, pp. 1669–1675 (2001)Google Scholar
  19. Frank, S.A.: Foundations of Social Evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton (1998)Google Scholar
  20. Gintis, H., Alden Smith, E., Bowles, S.: Costly signaling and cooperation. Journal of Theoretical Biology 213, 103–119 (2001)MathSciNetCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gouzoules, H., Gouzoules, S.: Primate communication: By nature honest, or by experience wise? International Journal of Primatology 23(4), 821–847 (2002)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Grafen, A.: Biological signals as handicaps. Journal of Theoretical Biology 144, 517–546 (1990)MathSciNetCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Grafen, A.: Optimisation of inclusive fitness. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 238, 541–563 (2006)MathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  24. Grice, H.P.: Logic and conversation. In: Cole, P., Morgan, J. (eds.) Syntax and Semantics III: Speech Acts, pp. 41–58. Academic Press, New York (1975)Google Scholar
  25. Grim, P., Wardach, S., Beltrani, V.: Location, location, location: The importance of spatialization in modeling cooperation and communication. Interaction Studies 7(1), 43–78 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Guinet, C.: Predation behaviour of Killer Whales (Orcinus orca) around Crozet islands. Canadian Journal of Zoology 70, 1656–1667 (1992)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Haley, K.J., Fessler, D.M.T.: Nobody’s watching? Subtle cues affect generousity in an anonymous economic game. Evolution and Human Behavior 26(3), 245–256 (2005)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hamilton, W.D.: The genetical evolution of social behaviour. Journal of Theoretical Biology 7, 1–52 (1964)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hardin, G.: The tragedy of the commons. Science 162, 1243–1248 (1968)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hurd, P.L.: Communication in discrete action-response games. Journal of Theoretical Biology 174, 217–222 (1995)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hurford, J.R.: Origins of Meaning. Oxford University Press, Oxford (2007)Google Scholar
  32. Knight, C.: Ritual/speech coevolution: a solution to the problem of deception. In: Hurford, J.R., Studdert-Kennedy, M., Knight, C. (eds.) Approaches to the Evolution of Language, pp. 68–91. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1998)Google Scholar
  33. Lachmann, M., Szmad, S., Bergstrom, C.T.: Cost and conflict in animal signals and human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 98(23), 13189–13194 (2001)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Maynard Smith, J.: Fertility, mating behaviour and sexual selection in Drosophila subobscura. Journal of Genetics 54, 261–279 (1956)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Maynard Smith, J.: Group selection and kin selection. Nature 201, 1145–1147 (1964)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Maynard Smith, J.: Sexual selection and the handicap principle. Journal of Theoretical Biology 57, 239–242 (1976)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Maynard Smith, J.: Evolution and the Theory of Games. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1982)CrossRefzbMATHGoogle Scholar
  38. Maynard Smith, J., Harper, D.G.C.: Animal signals: Models and terminology. Journal of Theoretical Biology 177, 305–311 (1995)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Maynard Smith, J., Harper, D.G.C.: Animal Signals. Oxford University Press, Oxford (2003)Google Scholar
  40. Mealey, L., Daood, C., Krage, M.: Enhanced memory for faces of cheaters. Ethology and Sociobiology 17, 119–128 (1996)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Milinski, M., Semmann, D., Krambeck, H.-J.: Reputation helps solve the ‘tragedy of the commons’. Nature 415, 424–426 (2002)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Miller, G.F.: The Mating Mind. BCA, London (2000)Google Scholar
  43. Oda, R.: Biased face recognition in the prisoner’s dilemma games. Evolution and Human Behavior 18, 309–315 (1997)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Parker, G.A.: Assessment strategy and the evolution of animal conflicts. Journal of Theoretical Biology 47, 223–243 (1974)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Pentland, A.: Honest Signals: How they Shape our World. MIT Press, Cambridge (2008)Google Scholar
  46. Piazza, J., Bering, J.M.: Concerns about reputation via gossip promote generous allocations in an economic game. Evolution and Human Behavior 29, 172–178 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Power, C.: Old wives’ tales: The gossip hypothesis and the reliability of cheap signals. In: Hurford, J.R., Studdert-Kennedy, M., Knight, C. (eds.) Approaches to the Evolution of Language, pp. 111–129. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1998)Google Scholar
  48. Reby, D., McComb, K.: Anatomical constraints generate honesty: Acoustic cues to age and weight in the roars of Red Deer stags. Animal Behaviour 65, 317–329 (2003)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Rohwer, S.: The social significance of avian winter plumage variability. Evolution 29, 593–610 (1975)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Rohwer, S., Rohwer, F.C.: Status signalling in Harris’ Sparrows: experimental deceptions achieved. Animal Behaviour 26, 1012–1022 (1978)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. de Saussure, F.: Course in General Linguistics. McGraw-Hill, New York (1959)Google Scholar
  52. Scherer, K.R., Feldstein, S., Bond, R.N., Rosenthal, R.: Vocal cues to deception: A comparative channel approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 14, 409–425 (1985)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Scott-Phillips, T.C.: The social evolution of language, and the language of social evolution. Evolutionary Psychology 5(4), 740–753 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Scott-Phillips, T.C.: On the correct application of animal signalling theory to human communication. In: Smith, A.D.M., Smith, K., Ferreri Cancho, R. (eds.) The Evolution of Language: Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on the Evolution of Language, pp. 275–282. World Scientific, Singapore (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Searcy, W.A., Nowicki, S.: The Evolution of Animal Communication. Princeton University Press, Princeton (2007)Google Scholar
  56. Silk, J.B., Kaldor, E., Boyd, R.: Cheap talk when interests conflict. Animal Behaviour 59, 423–432 (2000)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Skyrms, B.: The Evolution of the Social Contract. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1996)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Spence, M.: Job market signalling. Quarterly Journal of Economics 87, 355–374 (1973)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Sperber, D., Wilson, D.: Relevance: Communication and Cognition, 2nd edn. Blackwell, Oxford (1995)Google Scholar
  60. Sperber, D., Wilson, D.: Epistemic Vigilance. Paper presented at the Workshop on pragmatics and social cognition, UCL, London (2008)Google Scholar
  61. Szmad, S., Szathmry, E.: Selective scenarios for the emergence of natural language. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 21(10), 555–561 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Taylor, P.W., Hasson, O., Clark, D.L.: Body postures and patterns as amplifiers of physical condition. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, series B 267, 917–922 (2000)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Tooby, J., Cosmides, L.: The past explains the present: Emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethology and Sociobiology 11, 375–424 (1990)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Tricks of the traders, 3rd edn. Guardian Magazine (2008)Google Scholar
  65. Trivers, R.L.: The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology 46, 35–57 (1971)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Ulbaek, I.: The origin of language and cognition. In: Hurford, J.R., Studdert-Kennedy, M., Knight, C. (eds.) Approaches to the Evolution of Language, pp. 30–43. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1998)Google Scholar
  67. van Rooij, R.: Being Polite is a Handicap: Towards a Game Theoretical Analysis of Polite Linguistic Behaviour. Paper presented at the 9th conference on the theoretical aspects of rationality and knowledge (2003)Google Scholar
  68. Veblen, T.: The Theory of the Leisure Class. MacMillian, London (1899)Google Scholar
  69. Versluis, M., Schmitz, B., von der Heydt, A., Lohse, D.: How Snapping Shrimps snap: Through cavitating bubbles. Science 289, 2114–2117 (2000)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Wedekind, C., Milinski, M.: Cooperation through image scoring in humans. Science 288, 850–852 (2000)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. West, S.A., Gardner, A., Shuker, D.M., Reynolds, T., Burton-Chellow, M., Sykes, E.M., et al.: Coooperation and the scale and competition in humans. Current Biology 16, 1103–1106 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. West, S.A., Griffin, A.S., Gardner, A.: Social semantics: Altruism, cooperation, mutualism and strong reciprocity. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 20, 415–432 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Whitfield, D.P.: Plumage variability, status signalling and individual recognition in avian flocks. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 2, 13–18 (1987)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Yamagishi, T., Tanida, S., Mashima, R., Shimoma, S., Kanazawa, S.: You can judge a book by its cover: Evidence that cheaters may look different from co-operators. Evolution and Human Behavior 24, 290–301 (2003)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Zahavi, A.: Mate selection: A selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology 53, 205–214 (1975)CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  • Thomas C. Scott-Phillips
    • 1
  1. 1.Language Evolution and Computation Research Unit School of Psychology, Philosophy and Language SciencesUniversity of EdinburghUK

Personalised recommendations