Mind the Gap pp 223-244 | Cite as

From Grooming to Giving Blood: The Origins of Human Altruism

Chapter

Abstract

Cooperation plays an important role in the lives of most primates, including ourselves. However, the magnitude and scope of cooperation varies considerably across taxa: callitrichids pool efforts to rear a pairs’ offspring, male langurs jointly challenge resident males for access to groups of females, female baboons groom one another equitably, and male chimpanzees exchange support for mating opportunities. All of these forms of cooperation have analogs in human societies, but humans cooperate in more diverse contexts, with a wider range of partners, and at larger scales than other primates. The evolutionary foundations of cooperation in nonhuman primates – kinship, reciprocity, and mutualism – also generate cooperation in human societies, but cooperation in human societies may also be supported by group-level processes that do not exist in other primate species. The human capacities for culture may have created novel evolutionary forces that altered the selective benefits derived from cooperation.

References

  1. Anderson KG, Kaplan H, Lancaster JB (1999a) Paternal care by genetic fathers and stepfathers. I. Reports from Albuquerque men. Evol Hum Behav 20:405–431Google Scholar
  2. Anderson KG, Kaplan H, Lam D, Lancaster JB (1999b) Paternal care by genetic fathers and stepfathers. II. Reports by Xhosa high school students. Evol Hum Behav 20:433–451Google Scholar
  3. Aureli F, van Schaik CP (1991) Post conflict behaviour in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fasicularis). II. Coping with the uncertainty. Ethology 89:101–114Google Scholar
  4. Axelrod R (1986) An evolutionary approach to norms. Am Polit Sci Rev 80:1095–1111Google Scholar
  5. Axelrod R, Hamilton WD (1981) The evolution of cooperation. Science 211:1390–1396PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Barrett L, Henzi SP (2002) Constraints on relationship formation among female primates. Behaviour 139:263–289Google Scholar
  7. Barrett L, Henzi SP (2005) Monkeys, markets and minds: biological markets and primate sociality. In: Kappeler PM, van Schaik CP (eds) Cooperation in primates and humans: mechanisms and evolution. Springer, Berlin, pp 209–232Google Scholar
  8. Barrett L, Henzi SP, Weingrill T, Lycett J, Hill RA (1999) Market forces predict grooming reciprocity in female baboons. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 266:665–670Google Scholar
  9. Blaustein AR, Waldman B (1992) Kin recognition in anuran amphibians. Anim Behav 44:207–221Google Scholar
  10. Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  11. Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1988) The evolution of reciprocity in sizable groups. J Theor Biol 132:337–356PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1992) Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizable groups. Ethol Sociobiol 13:171–195Google Scholar
  13. Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1996) Why culture is common, but cultural evolution is rare. Proc Br Acad 88:77–93Google Scholar
  14. Boyd R, Richerson PJ (2002) Group beneficial norms spread rapidly in a structured population. J Theor Biol 215:287–296PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Brosnan SF, Silk JB, Henrich J, Mareno MC, Lambeth SP, Schapiro SJ (in press) Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) do not develop contingent reciprocity in an experimental task. Anim CognGoogle Scholar
  16. Brown GR, Almond REA, van Bergen Y (2004) Begging, stealing, and offering: food transfer in nonhuman primates. Adv Study Behav 34:265–295Google Scholar
  17. Burkart JM, Fehr E, Efferson C, van Schaik CP (2007) Other-regarding preferences in a nonhuman primate: common marmosets provision food altruistically. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:19762–19766PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Castles DL, Whiten A (1998) Post-conflict behaviour of wild olive baboons. II. Stress and self-directed behaviour. Ethology 104:148–160Google Scholar
  19. Chapais B, Berman CM (2007) Kinship and behavior in primates. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  20. Charpentier MJE, Peignot P, Hossaert-Mckey M, Wickings EJ (2007) Kin discrimination in juvenile mandrills, Mandrillus sphinx. Anim Behav 73:37–45Google Scholar
  21. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (1990) Attending to behaviour versus attending to knowledge: examining monkeys’ attribution of mental states. Anim Behav 40:742–753Google Scholar
  22. Cronin KA, Snowdon CT (2008) The effects of unequal reward distributions on cooperative problem solving by cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Anim Behav 75:245–257PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Cronin KA, Schroeder KKE, Rothwell ES, Silk JB, Snowdon CT (in press) Cooperatively breeding cottontop tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) do not donate rewards to their long-term mates. J Comp PsycholGoogle Scholar
  24. de Waal FBM (1997) The chimpanzee’s service economy: food for grooming. Evol Hum Behav 18:375–386Google Scholar
  25. de Waal FBM (2000) Attitudinal reciprocity in food sharing among brown capuchin monkeys. Anim Behav 60:253–261PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. de Waal FBM (2003) On the possibility of animal empathy. In: Manstead T, Frijda N, Fischer A (eds) Feelings and emotions: the Amsterdam symposium. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 379–399Google Scholar
  27. de Waal FBM, Berger ML (2000) Payment for labour in monkeys. Nature 404:563PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. de Waal FBM, van Roosmalen A (1979) Reconciliation and consolation among chimpanzees. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 5:55–66Google Scholar
  29. de Waal FBM, Leimgruber K, Greenberg AR (2008) Giving is self-rewarding for monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105:13685–13689PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Dietz JM (2004) Kinship structure and reproductive skew in cooperatively breeding primates. In: Chapais B, Berman CM (eds) Kinship and behavior in primates. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 223–241Google Scholar
  31. Duffy KG, Wrangham RW, Silk JB (2007) Male chimpanzees exchange political support for mating opportunities. Curr Biol 17:R586–R587Google Scholar
  32. Fehr E, Fischbacher U (2003) The nature of human altruism. Nature 425:785–791PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Frank RE, Silk JB (2009a) Impatient traders or contingent reciprocators? Evidence for the extended time course of grooming exchanges in baboons. Behaviour 136:1123–1135Google Scholar
  34. Frank RE, Silk JB (2009b) Grooming exchange between mothers and non-mothers: the price of natal attraction in wild baboons (Papio anubis). Behaviour 136:889–906Google Scholar
  35. Fraser ON, Stahl D, Aureli F (2008) Stress reduction through consolation in chimpanzees. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105:8557–8662PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. French JA (1997) Proximate regulation of singular breeding in callitrichid primates. In: Solomon NG, French JA (eds) Cooperative breeding in mammals. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 34–75Google Scholar
  37. Gamboa GJ (2004) Kin recognition in eusocial wasps. Ann Zool Fennici 41:789–808Google Scholar
  38. Gomes CM, Mundry R, Boesch C (2009) Long-term reciprocation of grooming in wild West African chimpanzees. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 276:699–706Google Scholar
  39. Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe: patterns of behavior. Belknap Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  40. Gros-Louis J (2004) The function of food-associated calls in white-faced capuchin monkeys, Cebus capucinus, from the perspective of the signaller. Anim Behav 67:431–440Google Scholar
  41. Gurven M (2004) To give and to give not: the behavioral ecology of human food transfers. Behav Brain Sci 27:543–583Google Scholar
  42. Gurven M (2006) The evolution of contingent cooperation. Curr Anthropol 47:185–192Google Scholar
  43. Haley KJ, Fessler DMT (2005) Nobody’s watching? Subtle cues affect generosity in an anonymous economic game. Evol Hum Behav 26:245–256Google Scholar
  44. Hamilton WD (1964) The genetical evolution of social behavior. I and II. J Theor Biol 7:1–52PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Hauser MD, Marler P (1993a) Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). I. Socioecological factors. Behav Ecol 4:194–205Google Scholar
  46. Hauser MD, Marler P (1993b) Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). II. Costs and benefits of call production and suppression. Behav Ecol 4:206–212Google Scholar
  47. Hauser MD, Chen MK, Chen F, Chuang E (2003) Give unto others: genetically unrelated cotton-top tamarins monkeys preferentially give food to those who altruistically give food back. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 270:2363–2370Google Scholar
  48. Hemelrijk CK (1994) Support for being groomed in long-tailed macaques, Macaca fasicularis. Anim Behav 48:479–481Google Scholar
  49. Henrich J, Boyd R (1998) The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. Evol Hum Behav 19:215–241Google Scholar
  50. Henrich J, McElreath R, Barr A, Ensminger J, Barrett C, Bolyanatz A, Cardenas JC, Gurven M, Gwako E, Henrich N, Lesorogol C, Marlowe F, Tracer D, Ziker J (2006) Costly punishment across human societies. Science 312:1767–1770PubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Henzi SP, Barrett L (2002) Infants as a commodity in a baboon market. Anim Behav 63:915–921Google Scholar
  52. Holmes WG, Mateo JM (2007) Kin recognition in rodents: issues and evidence. In: Wolff JO, Sherman PW (eds) Rodent societies: an ecological and evolutionary perspective. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 216–228Google Scholar
  53. Hrdy SB (2005) Evolutionary context of human development: the cooperative breeding model. In: Carter CS, Ahnert L, Grossman KE, Hrdy SB, Lamb ME, Porgest SW, Sacher N (eds) Attachment and bonding: a new synthesis. MIT, Boston, MA, pp 9–32Google Scholar
  54. Jensen K, Hare B, Call J, Tomasello M (2006) What’s in it for me? Self-regard precludes altruism and spite in chimpanzees. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 273:1013–1021Google Scholar
  55. Kaplan HS, Hill KR, Lancaster JB, Hurtado AM (2000) A theory of human life history evolution: diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evol Anthropol 9:156–185Google Scholar
  56. Kaplan H, Lancaster JB, Robson A (2003) Embodied capital and the evolutionary economics of the human lifespan. In: Carey JR, Tuljapurkar S (eds) Life span: evolutionary, ecological and demographic perspectives. The Population Council, New York, Population and Development Review 29(Suppl):152–182Google Scholar
  57. Komdeur J, Hatchwell BJ (1999) Kin recognition: function and mechanism in avian societies. Trends Ecol Evol 14:237–241PubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Koski SE, Sterck EHM (2007) Triadic postconflict affiliation in captive chimpanzees: does consolation console? Anim Behav 73:133–142Google Scholar
  59. Lakshminarayanan VR, Santos LR (2008) Capuchin monkeys are sensitive to others’ welfare. Curr Biol 18:R999–R1000Google Scholar
  60. Langergraber KE, Mitani JC, Vigilant L (2007) The limited impact of kinship on cooperation in wild chimpanzees. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:7786–7790PubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Lehmann J, Fickenscher G, Boesch C (2006) Kin biased investment in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 143:931–955Google Scholar
  62. Maestripieri D (1994) Social structure, infant handling, and mother styles in group-living Old World monkeys. Int J Primatol 15:531–553Google Scholar
  63. Manson JH (1999) Infant handling in wild Cebus capucinus: testing bonds between females? Anim Behav 57:911–921PubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Marlowe F (1999a) Male care and mating effort among Hadza foragers. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 46:57–64Google Scholar
  65. Marlowe F (1999b) Showoffs or providers? The parenting effort of Hadza men. Evol Hum Behav 20:391–404Google Scholar
  66. Melis AP, Hare B, Tomasello M (2008) Do chimpanzees reciprocate received favours? Anim Behav 76:951–962Google Scholar
  67. Mitani JC (2005) Reciprocal exchange in chimpanzees and other primates. In: Kappeler PM, van Schaik CP (eds) Cooperation in primates and humans: mechanisms and evolution. Springer, Berlin, pp 107–119Google Scholar
  68. Mitani JC (2009) Male chimpanzees form enduring and equitable social bonds. Anim Behav 77:633–640Google Scholar
  69. Muller MN, Mitani JC (2005) Conflict and cooperation in wild chimpanzees. Adv Study Behav 35:275–331Google Scholar
  70. Nishida T (1979) The social structure of chimpanzees of the Mahale mountains. In: Hamburg DA, McCown ER (eds) The great apes. Benjamin Cummings, Menlo Park, CA, pp 73–121Google Scholar
  71. Noë R (2005) Digging for the roots of trading. In: Kappeler PM, van Schaik CP (eds) Cooperation in primates and humans: evolution and mechanisms. Springer, Berlin, pp 233–261Google Scholar
  72. Noë R (2006) Cooperation experiments: coordination through communication versus acting apart together. Anim Behav 71:1–18Google Scholar
  73. Nowak MA, Sigmund K (1998) Evolution of indirect reciprocity by image scoring. Nature 393:573–577PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Palagi E, Cordoni G, Borgognini Tarli S (2006) Possible roles of consolation in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Am J Phys Anthropol 129:105–111PubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Panchanathan K, Boyd R (2004) Indirect reciprocity can stabilize cooperation without the second-order free rider problem. Nature 432:499–502PubMedGoogle Scholar
  76. Perry S, Manson JH, Muniz L, Gros-Louis J, Vigilant L (2008) Kin-biased social behaviour in wild adult female white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus. Anim Behav 76:187–199Google Scholar
  77. Preston SD, de Waal FBM (2002) Empathy: its ultimate and proximate bases. Behav Brain Sci 25:1–72PubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Price GR (1970) Selection and covariance. Nature 227:520–521PubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Pusey AE, Packer C (1987) Dispersal and philopatry. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 250–266Google Scholar
  80. Richerson PJ, Boyd R (2005) Not by genes alone: how culture transformed human evolution. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  81. Richerson PJ, Boyd R, Bettinger RL (2001) Was agriculture impossible during the Pleistocene but mandatory during the Holocene? A climate change hypothesis. Am Antiq 66:387–411Google Scholar
  82. Rogers AR (1990) Group selection by selective emigration: the effects of migration and kin structure. Am Nat 135:398–413Google Scholar
  83. Schino G (2001) Grooming, competition and social rank among female primates: a meta-analysis. Anim Behav 62:265–271Google Scholar
  84. Schino G, Aureli F (2008) Grooming reciprocation among female primates: a meta-analysis. Biol Lett 4:9–11PubMedGoogle Scholar
  85. Schino G, Gemiani S, Rosati L, Aureli F (2004) Behavioral and emotional response of Japanese macaque (Macaca fuscata) mothers after their offspring receive an aggression. J Comp Psychol 118:340–346PubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. Schino G, Di Giuseppe F, Visalberghi E (2009) The time frame of partner choice in the grooming reciprocation of Cebus apella. Ethology 115:70–76Google Scholar
  87. Seyfarth RM (1977) A model of social grooming among adult female monkeys. J Theor Biol 65:671–698PubMedGoogle Scholar
  88. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1984) Grooming, alliances, and reciprocal altruism in vervet monkeys. Nature 308:541–543PubMedGoogle Scholar
  89. Silk JB (2002) Kin selection in primate groups. Int J Primatol 23:849–875Google Scholar
  90. Silk JB (2005) Practicing Hamilton’s rule: kin selection in primate groups. In: Kappeler PM, van Schaik CP (eds) Cooperation in primates and humans: mechanisms and evolution. Springer, Berlin, pp 25–46Google Scholar
  91. Silk JB (2007a) The strategic dynamics of cooperation in primate groups. Adv Study Behav 37:1–42Google Scholar
  92. Silk JB (2007b) Empathy, sympathy, and prosocial preferences in primates. In: Dunbar RIM, Barrett L (eds) Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 115–126Google Scholar
  93. Silk JB, Brosnan SF, Vonk J, Henrich J, Povinelli DJ, Richardson AS, Lambeth SP, Mascaro J, Schapiro SJ (2005) Chimpanzees are indifferent to the welfare of unrelated group members. Nature 437:1357–1359PubMedGoogle Scholar
  94. Silk JB, Altmann J, Alberts SC (2006a) Social relationships among adult female baboons (Papio cynocephalus). I. Variation in the strength of social bonds. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 61:183–195Google Scholar
  95. Silk JB, Alberts SC, Altmann J (2006b) Social relationships among adult female baboons (Papio cynocephalus). II. Variation in the quality and stability of social bonds. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 61:197–204Google Scholar
  96. Smith KL, Alberts SC, Altmann J (2003) Wild female baboons bias their social behaviour towards paternal half-sisters. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 270:503–510Google Scholar
  97. Stevens JR, Hauser MD (2004) Why be nice? Psychological constraints on the evolution of cooperation. Trends Cogn Sci 8:60–65PubMedGoogle Scholar
  98. Stevens JR, Cushman FA, Hauser MD (2005) Evolving the psychological mechanisms for cooperation. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 36:499–518Google Scholar
  99. Tardif SD (1997) The bioenergetics of parental behavior and the evolution of alloparental care in marmosets and tamarins. In: Solomon NG, French JA (eds) Cooperative breeding in mammals. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 11–33Google Scholar
  100. Tomasello M, Call J (1997) Primate cognition. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  101. Trivers RL (1971) The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q Rev Biol 46:35–57Google Scholar
  102. Vonk J, Brosnan SF, Silk JB, Henrich J, Richardson AS, Lambeth SP, Schapiro SJ, Povinelli DJ (2008) Chimpanzees do not take advantage of very low cost opportunities to deliver food to unrelated group members. Anim Behav 75:1757–1770Google Scholar
  103. Warneken F, Tomasello M (2006) Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science 311:1301–1303PubMedGoogle Scholar
  104. Warneken F, Hare B, Melis AP, Hanus D, Tomasello M (2007) Spontaneous altruism by chimpanzees and young children. PLoS Biol 5:1414–1420Google Scholar
  105. Watts DP (2002) Reciprocity and interchange in the social relationships of wild male chimpanzees. Behaviour 139:343–370Google Scholar
  106. Watts DP, Mitani JC (2001) Boundary patrols and intergroup encounters in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 138:299–327Google Scholar
  107. Widdig A (2007) Paternal kin discrimination: the evidence and likely mechanisms. Biol Rev 82:319–334PubMedGoogle Scholar
  108. Widdig A, Nürnberg P, Krawczak M, Streich WJ, Bercovitch FB (2001) Paternal relatedness and age proximity regulate social relationships among adult female rhesus macaques. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:13769–13773PubMedGoogle Scholar
  109. Widdig A, Nürnberg P, Krawczak M, Streich WJ, Bercovitch FB (2002) Affiliation and aggression among adult female rhesus macaques: a genetic analysis of paternal cohorts. Behaviour 139:371–391Google Scholar
  110. Widdig A, Streich WJ, Nürnberg P, Croucher PJP, Bercovitch FB, Krawczak M (2006) Paternal kin bias in the agonistic interventions of adult female rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 61:205–214Google Scholar
  111. Winterhalder B (1986) Diet choice, risk, and food sharing in a stochastic environment. J Anthropol Archaeol 5:369–392Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of AnthropologyUniversity of CaliforniaLos AngelesUSA

Personalised recommendations