Degradation in Spatial Knowledge Acquisition When Using Automatic Navigation Systems

  • Avi Parush
  • Shir Ahuvia
  • Ido Erev
Part of the Lecture Notes in Computer Science book series (LNCS, volume 4736)

Abstract

Over-reliance on automated navigation systems may cause users to be “mindless” of the environment and not develop the spatial knowledge that maybe required when automation fails. This research focused on the potential degradation in spatial knowledge acquisition due to the reliance on automatic wayfinding systems. In addition, the impact of “keeping the user in the loop” strategies on spatial knowledge was examined. Participants performed wayfindings tasks in a virtual building with continuous or by-request position indication, in addition to responding to occasional orientation quizzes. Findings indicate that having position indication by request and orientation quizzes resulted in better acquired spatial knowledge. The findings are discussed in terms of keeping the user actively investing mental effort in the wayfinding task as a strategy to reduce the possible negative impact of automated navigation systems.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. 1.
    Allen, G.L.: Spatial abilities, cognitive maps and wayfinding: Bases for individual differences in spatial cognition and behavior. In: Golledge, R.G. (ed.) Wayfinding behavior: Cognitive mapping and other spatial processes, pp. 46–80. Johns-Hopkins Press, Baltimore, MD (1999)Google Scholar
  2. 2.
    Darken, R.P., Sibert, J.L.: Navigating in large virtual worlds. International Journal of Human Computer Interaction 8(1), 49–72 (1996)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. 3.
    Thorndyke, P.W., Hays-Roth, B.: Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation. Cognitive Psychology 14, 560–589 (1982)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. 4.
    Ruddle, R., Payne, S.J., Jones, D.M.: Navigating buildings in desk-top virtual environments: Experimental investigations using extended navigational experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 3, 143–159 (1997)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    Moeser, S.D.: Cognitive mapping in a complex building. Environment and Behavior 20(1), 21–49 (1986)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Hintzman, D.L., O’Dell, C.S., Arndt, D.R.: Orientation in cognitive maps. Cognitive Psychology 13, 149–206 (1981)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. 7.
    Golledge, R.G., Spector, N.A.: Comprehending the urban environment: Theory and practice. Geographical Analysis 14, 305–325 (1978)Google Scholar
  8. 8.
    Taylor, A.H., Tversky, B.: Descriptions and depictions of environments. Memory & Cognition 20, 483–496 (1992)Google Scholar
  9. 9.
    Taylor, A.H., Tversky, B.: Spatial mental models derived from survey and route descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language 31, 261–292 (1992)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    Taylor, A.H., Tversky, B.: Perspectives in spatial descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language 35, 371–391 (1996)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Wickens, C.D., Hollands, J.G.: Engineering Psychology and Human Performance, 3rd edn. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs (1999)Google Scholar
  12. 12.
    Schneider, L.F., Taylor, H.A.: How do you get there from here? Mental representations of route descriptions. Applied Cognitive Psychology 13, 415–441 (1999)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Lobben, A.K.: Tasks, Strategies, and Cognitive processes Associated with Navigational Map Reading: A Review Perspective. The Professional Geographer 56(2), 270–281 (2004)Google Scholar
  14. 14.
    Willis, K.: Mind the Gap: Mobile Applications and Wayfinding. In: Workshop for User Experience Design for Pervasive Computing, Pervasive 2005 (2005) Google Scholar
  15. 15.
    Leggat, A.P., Noyes, J.M.: Navigation Aids: Effects on Crew Workload and Performance. Military Psychology 12(2), 89–104 (2000)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. 16.
    Marston, J.R., Loomis, J.M., Klatzky, R.L., Golledge, R.G., Smith, E.L.: Evaluation of Spatial Displays for Navigation without Sight. ACM Transactions on Applied Perception 2(3), 110–124 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. 17.
    Wickens, C.D.: An introduction to human factors engineering. Addison-Wesley, Reading (1997)Google Scholar
  18. 18.
    Parasuraman, R.: Designing automation for human use: Empirical studies and quantitative models. Ergonomics 43(7), 931–951 (2000)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. 19.
    Stanton, N.A., Young, M.S.: Vehicle automation and driving performance. Ergonomics 41(7), 1014–1028 (1998)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. 20.
    Sarter, N.B., Woods, D.D., Billings, C.E.: Automation surprises. In: Salvendy, G. (ed.) Handbook of human factors and ergonomics, 2nd edn. pp. 1926–1943. John Wiley Sons, Chichester (1997)Google Scholar
  21. 21.
    Sarter, N.B., Woods, D.D.: Pilot interaction with cockpit automation II: An experimental study of pilot’s model and awareness of the flight management and guidance system. International Journal of Aviation Psychology 4(1), 1–28 (1994)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. 22.
    Tversky, A., Kahnman, D.: Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science 185, 1124–1131 (1984)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. 23.
    Parasuraman, R.: Humans and automation: use, misuse, disuse, abuse. Human Factors 39(2), 230–253 (1997)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. 24.
    Endsley, M.R., Kiris, E.O.: The out-of-the-loop performance problem and level of control in automation. Human factors 37(2), 381–394 (1995)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. 25.
    Johanson, G.: Stress, autonomy, and the maintenance of skills in supervisory control of automated systems. Applied Psychology: an international review 38(1), 45–56 (1989)Google Scholar
  26. 26.
    Wiener, E.L.: Cockpit Automation. In: Wiener, E.L., Nagel, D.C. (eds.) Human factors in aviation, pp. 433–461. Academic, San Diego (1988)Google Scholar
  27. 27.
    Johansson, G., Cavalini, P., Pettersson, P.: Psychobiological Reactions to Unpredictable Performance Stress in a Monotonous Situation. Human Performance 9(4), 363–384 (1996)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. 28.
    Aslan, I., Schwalm, M., Baus, J., Kruger, A., Schwartz, T.: Acquisition of Spatial Knowledge in Location Aware Mobile Pedestrian Navigation Systems. In: MobileHCI 2006, pp. 105–108. ACM Press, New York (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. 29.
    Burnett, G.E., Lee, K.: The effect of vehicle navigation systems on the formation of cognitive maps. In: Underwood, G. (ed.) Traffic and Transport Psychology: Theory and Application, pp. 407–418. Elsevier, Amsterdam (2005)Google Scholar
  30. 30.
    Jarus, T.: Is more always better? Optimal amounts of feedback in learning to calibrate sensory awareness. The Occupational Therapy Journal of Research 15(3), 181–197 (1995)Google Scholar
  31. 31.
    Salmoni, A.W., Schmidt, R.A., Walter, C.B.: Knowledge of results and motor learning: A review and critical reappraisal. Psychological Bulletin 95(3), 355–386 (1984)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. 32.
    Sparrow, W.A., Summers, J.J.: Performance on trials without knowledge of results (KR) in reduced relative frequency presentations of KR. Journal of Motor Behavior 24(2), 197–209 (1992)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. 33.
    Parush, A., Berman, D.: Orientation and navigation in 3D user interfaces: The impact of navigation aids and landmarks. International Journal of Human Computer Studies 61(3), 375–395 (2004)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. 34.
    Salomon, G.: Television Is Easy and Print Is Tough The Differences Investment of Mental Effort in Learning as a Function of Perceptions and Attributions. Journal of Educational Psychology 76(4), 647–658 (1984)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. 35.
    Cuevas, H.M., Fiore, S.M., Bowers, C.A., Salas, E.: Fostering constructive cognitive and metacognitive activity in computer-based complex task training environments. Computers in Human Behavior 20, 225–241 (2004)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. 36.
    Hull, C.S.: The principles of behavior. Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York (1943)Google Scholar
  37. 37.
    Estes, W.K.: Statistical theory of distributional phenomena in learning. Psychological Review 62, 369–377 (1955)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. 38.
    Bjork, R.A., Bjork, E.L.: A new theory of disuse and an old theory of fluctuation. In: Healy, A., Kosslyn, S., Shiffrin, R. (eds.) From learning process to cognitive processes: Essays in honor of William K. Estes, vol. 2, pp. 35–67. Earlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ (1992)Google Scholar
  39. 39.
    Richardson, A.E., Montello, D.R., Hegarty, M.: Spatial knowledge acquisition from maps and from navigation in real and virtual environments. Memory Cognition 27(4), 741–750 (1999)Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Avi Parush
    • 1
    • 2
  • Shir Ahuvia
    • 2
  • Ido Erev
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of Psychology, Carleton University, B552 Loeb Building, 1125 Colonel By Drive, Ottawa, ON , K1S 5B6Canada
  2. 2.Faculty of Industrial Management and Engineering, Israel Institute of Technology, Haifa, 32000Israel

Personalised recommendations