Dynamics of Interpersonal Coordination

  • Richard C. Schmidt
  • Michael J. Richardson

Abstract

Everyday human actions often occur in a social context. Past psychological research has found that the motor behavior of socially situated individuals tends to be coordinated. Our research performed over the last 20 years has sought to understand how the mutuality, accommodation, and synchrony found in everyday interactional coordination can be understood using a dynamical theory of behavioral order, namely coordination dynamics. Using laboratory interpersonal tasks, we have demonstrated that when two people are asked to rhythmically coordinate their limbs they show behavioral phenomena identical to those found in bimanual interlimb coordination, which has been mathematically modeled as a dynamical process. Research has demonstrated that these same dynamical organizing principles can coordinate the rhythmic movements of two people unintentionally and that the weaker, intermittent coordination that ensues is affected by both perceptual (e.g., attentional focus and information pickup activity of the visual system) and dynamical constraints (e.g., intrapersonal rhythmic synergies and period basin of entrainment). Other research has investigated how traditional social and personality properties of a dyad (rapport, social competence) relate to dynamical properties of a dyad’s coordinated movements and how the stability of coordinated movements mirrors the stability of mental connectedness experienced in social interactions.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. 1.
    Amazeen EL, Amazeen PG, Post AA, Beek PJ (1999) Timing the selection of information during rhythmic catching. J Motor Behav 31:279–289Google Scholar
  2. 2.
    Amazeen PG, Schmidt RC, Turvey MT (1995) Frequency detuning of the phase entrainment dynamics of visually coupled rhythmic movements. Biol Cybern 72:511–518CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. 3.
    Bang J (2007) Steps towards an ecological approach to thinking. Journal of Anthropological Psychology 18:2–13Google Scholar
  4. 4.
    Baron R (2007) Situating coordination and cooperation between ecological and social psychology. Ecol Psychol 19:179–199Google Scholar
  5. 5.
    Bernieri FJ (1988) Coordinated movement and rapport in teacher-student interactions. J Nonverbal Behav 12:120–138CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Bernieri FJ, Reznick JS, Rosenthal R (1988) Synchrony, pseudosynchrony, and dissynchrony: measuring the entrainment process in mother-infant interactions. J Pers Soc Psychol 54:243–253CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. 7.
    Bernieri FJ, Rosenthal R (1991) Interpersonal coordination: behavior matching and interactional synchrony. In: Feldman RS, Rime B (eds) Fundamentals of nonverbal behavior. Studies in emotion & social interaction. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp. 401–432Google Scholar
  8. 8.
    Boudreau LA (1992) Social competence as a determinant of the avoidance of categorization in forming interpersonal impressions. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Connecticut, 1991). Dissertation Abstracts International, 53, 3205BGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    Buck J, Buck E (1976) Synchronous fireflies. Sci Am 234:74–85CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    Byblow WD, Carson RG, Goodman D (1994) Expressions of asymmetries and anchoring in bimanual coordination. Hum Movement Sci 13:3–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Chui K (2005) Temporal patterning of speech and iconic gestures in conversational discourse. J Pragmatics 37:871–887CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.
    Clark A (1997) Being there: putting brain, body, and world together again. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Clark HH (1996) Using language. Cambridge University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  14. 14.
    Collins JJ (1999) Fishing for function in noise. Nature 402:241–242CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. 15.
    Collins JJ, Imhoff TT, Grigg P (1997) Noise-mediated enhancements and decrements in human tactile sensation. Phys Rev E 56:923–926CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. 16.
    Condon WS (1982) Cultural microrhythms. In: Davis M (ed) Interaction rhythms: periodicity in communicative behavior. Human Sciences Press, New York, pp. 53–77Google Scholar
  17. 17.
    Condon WS, Ogston WD (1966) Sound film analysis of normal and pathological behavior patterns. J Nerv Ment Dis 143:338–347CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. 18.
    Condon WS, Ogston WD (1967) A segmentation of behavior. J Psychiat Res 5:221–235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. 19.
    Condon WS, Sander LW (1974) Neonate movement is synchronized with adult speech: interactional participation and language acquisition. Science 183:99–101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. 20.
    Copeland J, Moiseff A (2004) Flash precision at the start of synchrony in Photoris frontalis. Integr Comp Biol 44:259–263CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. 21.
    Csikszentmihalyi M (1990) Flow: the psychology of optimal experience. Harper Collins, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  22. 22.
    Feldman R, Eidelman AI (2004) Parent-infant synchrony and the socialemotional development of triplets. Dev Psychol 40:1133–1147CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. 23.
    Foth E, Graham, D (1983) Influence of loading parallel to the body axis on the walking coordination of an insect. Biol Cybern 48:149–157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. 24.
    Franz EA, Zelaznik HN, Swinnen S, Walter C (2001) Spatial conceptual influences on the coordination of bimanual actions: when a dual task becomes a single task. J Motor Behav 33:103–112Google Scholar
  25. 25.
    Furuyama N (2002) Prolegomena of a theory of between-person coordination of speech and gesture. Int J Hum-Comput St 57:347–374CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. 26.
    Gibson JJ (1962) Observations on active touch. Psychol Rev 69:477–491CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. 27.
    Gibson JJ (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. HoughtonMifflin, BostonGoogle Scholar
  28. 28.
    Gilmore R (1979) Catastrophe time scales and conventions. Phys Rev A 20:2510–2515CrossRefMathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  29. 29.
    Gilmore R (1981) Catastrophe theory for scientists and engineers. Wiley & Sons, New YorkMATHGoogle Scholar
  30. 30.
    Goldfield EC, Schmidt RC, Fitzpatrick P (1999) Coordination dynamics of abdomen and chest during infant breathing: a comparison of full-term and preterm infants at 38 weeks postconceptional age. Ecol Psychol 11:209–232CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. 31.
    Grammer K, Kruck KB, Magnusson MS (1998) The courtship dance: Patterns of nonverbal synchronization in opposite-sex encounters. J Nonverbal Behav 22:3–29CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. 32.
    Hajnal A, Harrison SJ, Richardson MJ, Schmidt RC (2006) Visual information and environmental coordination. Paper presented at the North American Meeting of the International Society for Ecological Psychology, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OHGoogle Scholar
  33. 33.
    Haken H (1977/1983) Synergetics: an introduction. Springer-Verlag, BerlinMATHGoogle Scholar
  34. 34.
    Haken H (1983) Advanced synergetics. Springer-Verlag, BerlinMATHGoogle Scholar
  35. 35.
    Haken H, Kelso JAS, Bunz H (1985) A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements. Biol Cybern 51:347–356MATHCrossRefMathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  36. 36.
    Hanson FE (1978) Comparative studies of firefly pacemakers. Fed Proc 37:2158–2164Google Scholar
  37. 37.
    Henriques DYP, Crawford JD (2002) Role of eye, head, and shoulder geometry in the planning of accurate arm movements. J Neurophysiol 87:1677–1685Google Scholar
  38. 38.
    Hollands MA, Marple-Horvat DE, Henkes S, Rowan AK (1995) Human eye movements during visually guided stepping. J Motor Behav 27:155–163CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. 39.
    Isabella RA, Belsky J (1991) Interactional synchrony and the origins of infantmother attachment: a replication study. Child Dev 62:373–384CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. 40.
    Johnston L, Miles L, Richardson MJ, Schmidt RC, Marsh KL, Yabar Y (2006) Implicit mimicry and synchronization: impact of and on liking. Poster presented at the North American Meeting of the International Society for Ecological Psychology, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OHGoogle Scholar
  41. 41.
    Julien D, Brault M, Chartrand E, Begin J (2000) Immediacy behaviours and synchrony in satisfied and dissatisfied couples. Can J Beh Sci 32:84–90Google Scholar
  42. 42.
    Kelso JAS (1981) On the oscillatory basis of movement. B Psychonomic Soc 18:63Google Scholar
  43. 43.
    Kelso JAS (1984) Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. Am J Physiol: Reg, Integr Comp 246:R1000–R1004Google Scholar
  44. 44.
    Kelso JAS (1995) Dynamic patterns. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  45. 45.
    Kelso JAS, Delcolle JD, Schäner G (1990) Action-perception as a pattern formation process. In: Jeannerod M (ed) Attention and performance XIII, Vol. 5. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 139–169Google Scholar
  46. 46.
    Kelso JAS, Ding M (1994) Fluctuations, intermittency, and controllable chaos in biological coordination. In: Newell KM, Corcos DM (eds) Variability in motor control. Human Kinetics, Champaign, IL, pp. 291–316Google Scholar
  47. 47.
    Kelso JAS, Holt KG, Kugler PN, Turvey MT (1980) On the concept of coordinative structures as dissipative structures: II. Empirical lines of convergence. In: Stelmach GE, Requin J (eds) Tutorials in motor behavior. North-Holland, Amsterdam, pp. 49–70Google Scholar
  48. 48.
    Kelso JAS, Holt KG, Rubin P, Kugler PN (1981) Patterns of human interlimb coordination emerge from the properties of nonlinear limit cycle processes: Theory and data. J Motor Behav 13:226–261Google Scholar
  49. 49.
    Kelso JAS, Scholz JP, Schöner G (1986) Nonequilibrium phase transitions in coordinated biological motion: Critical fluctuations. Phys Lett 118:279–284CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. 50.
    Kendon A (1970) Movement coordination in social interaction: some examples. Acta Psychol 32:1–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. 51.
    Kugler PN, Kelso JAS, Turvey MT (1980) On the concept of coordinative structures as dissipative structures: I. Theoretical lines of convergence. In: Stelmach GE, Requin J (eds) Tutorials in motor behavior. North-Holland, Amsterdam, pp. 3–47Google Scholar
  52. 52.
    Kugler PN, Kelso JAS, Turvey MT (1982) On the control and coordination of naturally developing systems. In: Kelso JAS, Clark JE (eds) The development of movement control and coordination. Wiley, New York, pp. 5–78Google Scholar
  53. 53.
    Kugler PN, Turvey MT (1987) Information, natural law and the self-assembly of rhythmic movement. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJGoogle Scholar
  54. 54.
    Large EW, Jones MR (1999) The dynamics of attending: how people track time-varying events. Psychol Rev 106: 119–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. 55.
    Laurent M, Thomson JA (1988) The role of visual information in control of a constrained locomotor task. J Motor Behav 20:17–37Google Scholar
  56. 56.
    Lockman JJ, Hazen NL (1989) Action in a social context. Plenum, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  57. 57.
    Lopresti-Goodman SM, Silva PL, Richardson MJ, Schmidt RC (2006) Frequency basin of entrainment for unintentional coordination. Paper presented at the North American Meeting of the International Society for Ecological Psychology, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OHGoogle Scholar
  58. 58.
    Marsh KL, Richardson MJ, Baron RM, Schmidt RC (2006) Contrasting approaches to perceiving and acting with others. Ecol Psychol 18:1–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. 59.
    Patterson ML, Webb A, Schwartz W (2002) Passing encounters: Patterns of recognition and avoidance in pedestrians. Basic Appl Soc Psych 24:57–66Google Scholar
  60. 60.
    Richardson MJ, Curtis K, Zeising M, Schmidt RC (2006) Visual tracking and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Poster presented at the North American Meeting of the International Society for Ecological Psychology, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OHGoogle Scholar
  61. 61.
    Richardson MJ, Marsh KL, Isenhower R, Goodman J, Schmidt RC (2006) Rocking together: Dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Manuscript submitted for publicationGoogle Scholar
  62. 62.
    Richardson MJ, Marsh KL, Schmidt RC (2005) Effects of visual and verbal information on unintentional interpersonal coordination. J Exp Psychol: Human Percept Perform 31:62–79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. 63.
    Richardson MJ, Schmidt RC, Kay BA (2007) Distinguishing the noise and attractor strength of coordinated limb movements using recurrence analysis. Biol Cybern 96:59–78MATHCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. 64.
    Riggio R (1986) Assessment of basic social skills. J Pers Soc Psychol 51:649–660CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. 65.
    Riley MA, Turvey MT (2002) Variability and determinism in motor behavior. J Motor Behav 34:99–125CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. 66.
    Roerdink J, Peper CM, Beek PJ (2005) Effects of correct and transformed visual feedback on rhythmic visuo-motor tracking: tracking performance and visual search behavior. Hum Movement Sci 24:379–402CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. 67.
    Santvoord AAM, Beek PJ (1994) Phasing and the pickup of optical information in cascade juggling. Ecol Psychol 6:239–263CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. 68.
    Schmidt RC (1988) Dynamical constraints on the coordination of rhythmic limb movements between two people. Unpublished Ph. D. Thesis, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT.Google Scholar
  69. 69.
    Schmidt, RC (2007). Scaffolds for social meaning. Ecol Psychol 19:137–151Google Scholar
  70. 70.
    Schmidt RC, Bienvenu M, Fitzpatrick PA, Amazeen PG (1998) A comparison of intra-and interpersonal interlimb coordination: coordination breakdowns and coupling strength. J Exp Psychol: Human Percept Perform 24:884–900CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. 71.
    Schmidt RC, Carello C, Turvey MT (1990) Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. J Exp Psychol: Human Percept Perform 16:227–247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. 72.
    Schmidt RC, Christianson N, Carello C, Baron R (1994) Effects of social and physical variables on between-person visual coordination. Ecol Psychol 6:159–183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. 73.
    Schmidt RC, O’Brien B (1997) Evaluating the dynamics of unintended interpersonal coordination. Ecol Psychol 9:189–206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. 74.
    Schmidt RC, O’Brien B (1998) Modeling interpersonal coordination dynamics: Implications for a dynamical theory of developing systems. In: Molenaar PC, Newell K (eds) Dynamics Systems and Development: beyond the metaphor. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 221–240Google Scholar
  75. 75.
    Schmidt RC, Richardson MJ, Arsenault CA, Galantucci B (2007) Visual tracking and entrainment to an environmental rhythm. J Exp Psychol: Human Percept Perform 33:860–870CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. 76.
    Schmidt RC, Richardson MJ, Curtis K (2005) Effects of perceptual and production rhythms on unintentional synchronization. Poster presented at Progress in Motor Control V: A Multidisciplinary Perspective, Pennsylvania State University, State College, PA, August 2005Google Scholar
  77. 77.
    Schmidt RC, Shaw BK, Turvey MT (1993) Coupling dynamics in interlimb coordination. J Exp Psychol: Human Percept Perform 19(2):397–415CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. 78.
    Schmidt RC, Turvey MT. (1992) Long-term consistencies in assembling coordinated rhythmic movements. Hum Movement Sci 11:349–376CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. 79.
    Schmidt RC, Turvey MT (1994) Phase-entrainment dynamics of visually coupled rhythmic movements. Biol Cybern 70:369–376CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. 80.
    Schmidt RC, Turvey MT (1995) Models of interlimb coordination: equilibria, local analyses, and spectral patterning. J Exp Psychol: Human Percept Perform 21:432–443CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. 81.
    Schöner G, Haken H, Kelso JAS (1986) A stochastic theory of phase transitions in human hand movement. Biol Cybern 53:247–257MATHCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. 82.
    Shockley KD, Baker AA, Richardson MJ, Fowler CA (2007) Verbal constraints on interpersonal postural coordination. J Exp Psychol: Human Percept Perform 33:201–208CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. 83.
    Shockley K, Santana MV, Fowler CA (2003) Mutual interpersonal postural constraints are involved in cooperative conversation. J Exp Psychol: Human Percept Perform 29:326–332CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. 84.
    Stein PSG (1974) Neural control of interappendage phase during locomotion. Am Zool 14:1003–1016Google Scholar
  85. 85.
    Sternad D, Turvey MT, Schmidt RC (1992) Average phase difference theory and 1:1 phase entrainment in interlimb coordination. Biol Cybern 67:223–231CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. 86.
    Strogatz SH (1994) Nonlinear dynamic and chaos. Perseus, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  87. 87.
    Strogatz SH (2003) Sync: The emerging science of spontaneous order. Hyperion Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  88. 88.
    Tickle-Degnen L (2006) Nonverbal behavior and its functions in the ecosystem of rapport. In: Manusov V, Patterson M (eds) The SAGE Handbook of Nonverbal Communication. Sage, Thousand Oaks, CAGoogle Scholar
  89. 89.
    Turvey MT, Rosenblum LD, Schmidt RC, Kugler PN (1986) Fluctuations and phase symmetry in coordinated rhythmic movement. J Exp Psychol: Human Percept Perform 12:564–583CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. 90.
    van Donkelaar P (1997) Eye-hand interactions during goal-directed pointing movements. Neuroreport 8:2139–2142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. 91.
    von Holst E (1973) The collected papers of Eric von Holst. In: Martin R, Trans. The behavioral physiology of animal and man, Vol. 1. University of Miami Press, Coral Gables, FL (Original work published 1939)Google Scholar
  92. 92.
    Warren WH (1984) Perceiving affordances: visual guidance of stair climbing. J Exp Psychol: Human Percept Perform 10:683–703CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. 93.
    Warren WH, Yaffe DM (1989) Dynamics of step length adjustment during running: a comment on Patla, Robinson, Samways, Armstrong. J Exp Psychol: Human Percept Perform 15:618–623CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. 94.
    Wilson M (2002) Six views of embodied cognition. Psychon Bull Rev 9:625–636Google Scholar
  95. 95.
    Yarbus AL (1967) Eye movements and vision. Plenum Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  96. 96.
    Zanone PG, Kelso JAS (1990) Relative timing from the perspective of dynamic pattern theory: stability and instability. In J. Fagard, P. H. Wolff (eds) The development of timing control and temporal organization in coordinated action. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp. 69–92Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Richard C. Schmidt
    • 1
  • Michael J. Richardson
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyCollege of the Holy Cross and University of ConnecticutWorcesterUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyColby College and University of ConnecticutWatervilleUSA

Personalised recommendations