Advertisement

Understanding Empathy from Interactional Synchrony in Humans and Non-human Primates

  • Lira Yu
  • Yuko Hattori
  • Shinya Yamamoto
  • Masaki Tomonaga
Chapter
Part of the Interdisciplinary Evolution Research book series (IDER)

Abstract

Humans are highly responsive to social partners. Either consciously or unconsciously, humans adjust one’s own behavior to match with those of others. Moreover, humans can predict others’ intention or desire and often behave in a prosocial way to help themselves. A comparative approach is one of the powerful tools to understand the evolutionary origins of these social behaviors in humans, including emotional contagion, sympathetic concern, perspective-taking and targeted helping behaviors, and its relationship to each other. Among these social behaviors, an ability for the coordinated movements with others (i.e., interpersonal coordination) has been postulated as the most essential to the other social behaviors. Interpersonal coordination includes two types of behavior: mimicry and interactional synchrony. Mimicry is matching a type of the behavior, such as body postures or facial expressions. In contrast, interactional synchrony is matching the timing of behavior. Relative to the studies on mimicry, there are few studies on interactional synchrony in non-human primate species possibly due to the difficulties in establishing a methodology. In this chapter, we present gradually the increasing number of studies on interactional synchrony in humans and non-human primates. The findings demonstrate that the ability for interactional synchrony is shared across humans, chimpanzees, bonobos, and macaques. The latest findings from a direct comparison between humans and chimpanzees further demonstrate that there are both similarities and significant differences on the ability between two species. At the end of this chapter, we suggest a future direction of the comparative studies on interactional synchrony.

Keywords

Social interaction Rhythmic coordination Comparative cognition Empathy Synchrony 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We thank T. Matsuzawa and other staff members at the Language and Intelligence Section and Center for Human Evolution Modeling Research of Kyoto University Primate Research Institute for their support and daily care of the chimpanzees. We would like to acknowledge financial supports by Japan Society for the Promotion of Science Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research (244525 and 16F16001 to L.Y.; 16H01487 to Y.H.; 26118509, 15H05309, 15H01619, and 17H05862 to S.Y.; and 15H05709, 16H06283, 20002001, 23220006, and 24000001 to M.T.), JSPS-CCSN, Global COE programs (A06, D07), and the JSPS Leading Graduate Program in Primatology and Wildlife Science (U04) at Kyoto University.

References

  1. Allison T, Puce A, McCarthy G (2000) Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends Cogn Sci 4(7):267–278CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Anderson JR, Myowa-Yamakoshi M, Matsuzawa T (2004) Contagious yawning in chimpanzees. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 271(Suppl 6):S468–S470CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Arcadi AC (1996) Phrase structure of wild chimpanzee pant hoots: patterns of production and interpopulation variability. Am J Primatol 39(3):159–178CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bernieri FJ, Rosenthal R (1991) Interpersonal coordination: behavior matching and interactional synchrony. In: Feldman RS, Rimé B (eds) Fundamentals of nonverbal behavior. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 401–432Google Scholar
  5. Boesch C, Boesch H (2000) The chimpanzees of the Tai forest. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  6. Campbell MW, Carter JD, Proctor D, Eisenberg ML, de Waal FB (2009) Computer animations stimulate contagious yawning in chimpanzees. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.1087 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Chartrand TL, Bargh JA (1999) The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol 76(6):893–910CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cirelli LK, Einarson KM, Trainor LJ (2014) Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. Dev Sci 17(6):1003–1011CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. de Waal FBM (2008) Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution of empathy. Annu Rev Psychol 59:279–300CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. de Waal FBM (2009) The age of empathy: nature’s lessons for a kinder society. Harmony Books, New York, NYGoogle Scholar
  11. Dimberg U (1982) Facial reactions to facial expressions. Psychophysiology 19(6):643–647CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Dimberg U, Thunberg M, Elmehed K (2000) Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial expressions. Psychol Sci 11(1):86–89CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Fedurek P, Machanda ZP, Schel AM, Slocombe KE (2013) Pant hoot chorusing and social bonds in male chimpanzees. Anim Behav 86(1):189–196CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Ferrari PF, Visalberghi E, Paukner A, Fogassi L, Ruggiero A, Suomi SJ (2006) Neonatal imitation in rhesus macaques. PLoS Biol 4(9):e302CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  15. Frith CD, Frith U (1999) Interacting minds-a biological basis. Science 286(5445):1692–1695CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  17. Grèzes J, Fonlupt P, Bertenthal B, Delon-Martin C, Segebarth C, Decety J (2001) Does perception of biological motion rely on specific brain regions? NeuroImage 13(5):775–785CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Grossman ED, Donnelly M, Price R, Pickens D, Morgan V, Neighbor G, Blake R (2000) Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. J Cogn Neurosci 12(5):711–720CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Hattori Y, Tomonaga M, Matsuzawa T (2013) Spontaneous synchronized tapping to an auditory rhythm in a chimpanzee. Sci Rep 3:1566CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. Hattori Y, Tomonaga M, Matsuzawa T (2015) Distractor effect of auditory rhythms on self-paced tapping in chimpanzees and humans. PLoS One 10(7):e0130682CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Hove MJ, Risen JL (2009) It’s all in the timing: interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Soc Cogn 27(6):949–960CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Issartel J, Marin L, Cadopi M (2007) Unintended interpersonal co-ordination: “can we march to the beat of our own drum?”. Neurosci Lett 411(3):174–179CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Kilner JM, Paulignan Y, Blakemore SJ (2003) An interference effect of observed biological movement on action. Curr Biol 13(6):522–525CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Kirschner S, Tomasello M (2009) Joint drumming: social context facilitates synchronization in preschool children. J Exp Child Psychol 102(3):299–314CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Kirschner S, Tomasello M (2010) Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evol Human Behav 31(5):354–364CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Konoike N, Mikami A, Miyachi S (2012) The influence of tempo upon the rhythmic motor control in macaque monkeys. Neurosci Res 74(1):64–67CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Large EW, Gray PM (2015) Spontaneous tempo and rhythmic entrainment in a bonobo (Pan paniscus). J Comp Psychol 129(4):317CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Matsuzawa T, Tomonaga M, Tanaka M (2006) Cognitive development in chimpanzees. Springer, TokyoCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Meltzoff AN, Moore MK (1977) Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198(4312):75–78CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Merchant H, Honing H (2014) Are non-human primates capable of rhythmic entrainment? Evidence for the gradual audiomotor evolution hypothesis. Front Neurosci 7:274CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  31. Mitani JC, Gros-Louis J (1998) Chorusing and call convergence in chimpanzees: tests of three hypotheses. Behaviour 135:1041–1064CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Myowa-Yamakoshi M, Tomonaga M, Tanaka M, Matsuzawa T (2004) Imitation in neonatal chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Dev Sci 7(4):437–442CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Nagasaka Y, Chao ZC, Hasegawa N, Notoya T, Fujii N (2013) Spontaneous synchronization of arm motion between Japanese macaques. Sci Rep 3:1151CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  34. Néda Z, Ravasz E, Brechet Y, Vicsek T, Barabási A-L (2000) Self-organizing processes: the sound of many hands clapping. Nature 403(6772):849–850CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Oram MW, Perrett DI (1994) Responses of anterior superior temporal polysensory (STPa) neurons to biological motion stimuli. J Cogn Neurosci 6(2):99–116CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Oullier O, de Guzman GC, Jantzen KJ, Lagarde J, Kelso JAS (2008) Social coordination dynamics: measuring human bonding. Soc Neurosci 3(2):178–192CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  37. Patel AD (2006) Musical rhythm, linguistic rhythm, and human evolution. Music Percept Interdisc J 24(1):99–104CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Patel AD, Iversen JR, Bregman MR, Schulz I (2009) Experimental evidence for synchronization to a musical beat in a nonhuman animal. Curr Biol 19(10):827–830CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Paukner A, Anderson JR (2006) Video-induced yawning in stumptail macaques (Macaca arctoides). Biol Lett 2(1):36–38CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Repp BH, Penel A (2002) Auditory dominance in temporal processing: new evidence from synchronization with simultaneous visual and auditory sequences. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 28(5):1085CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Repp BH, Penel A (2004) Rhythmic movement is attracted more strongly to auditory than to visual rhythms. Psychol Res 68(4):252–270CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Richardson MJ, Marsh KL, Isenhower RW, Goodman JRL, Schmidt RC (2007) Rocking together: dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Hum Mov Sci 26(6):867–891CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Sato W, Yoshikawa S (2007) Spontaneous facial mimicry in response to dynamic facial expressions. Cognition 104(1):1–18CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Schmidt RC, O'Brien B (1997) Evaluating the dynamics of unintended interpersonal coordination. Ecol Psychol 9(3):189–206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Schmidt RC, Turvey MT (1994) Phase-entrainment dynamics of visually coupled rhythmic movements. Biol Cybern 70(4):369–376CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Schmidt RC, Carello C, Turvey MT (1990) Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 16(2):227CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Sebanz N, Bekkering H, Knoblich G (2006) Joint action: bodies and minds moving together. Trends Cogn Sci 10(2):70–76CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Van Baaren RB, Holland RW, Kawakami K, Van Knippenberg A (2004) Mimicry and prosocial behavior. Psychol Sci 15(1):71–74CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. van Ulzen NR, Lamoth CJ, Daffertshofer A, Semin GR, Beek PJ (2008) Characteristics of instructed and uninstructed interpersonal coordination while walking side-by-side. Neurosci Lett 432(2):88–93CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Wallin NL, Merker B, Brown S (2000) The origins of music. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  51. Watts DP, Mitani JC (2001) Boundary patrols and intergroup encounters in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 138(3):299–327CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Wiltermuth SS, Heath C (2009) Synchrony and cooperation. Psychol Sci 20(1):1–5CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Yamamoto S (2017) Primate empathy: three factors and their combinations for empathy-related phenomena. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci 8(3).  https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1431 Google Scholar
  54. Yamamoto S, Humle T, Tanaka M (2012) Chimpanzees’ flexible targeted helping based on an understanding of conspecifics’ goals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109(9):3588–3592CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Yu L, Tomonaga M (2015) Interactional synchrony in chimpanzees: examination through a finger-tapping experiment. Sci Rep 5:10218CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  56. Yu L, Tomonaga M (2016) Unidirectional adaptation in tempo in pairs of chimpanzees during simultaneous tapping movement: an examination under face-to-face setup. Primates 57:181–185CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Yu L, Tomonaga M (2018) Effect of visual cues in addition to moderate auditory cues on temporal coordination: a comparative study in humans and chimpanzees. bioRxiv:290379.  https://doi.org/10.1101/290379
  58. Yun K, Watanabe K, Shimojo S (2012) Interpersonal body and neural synchronization as a marker of implicit social interaction. Sci Rep 2:959CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  59. Zarco W, Merchant H, Prado L, Mendez JC (2009) Subsecond timing in primates: comparison of interval production between human subjects and rhesus monkeys. J Neurophysiol 102(6):3191–3202CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  60. Zivotofsky AZ, Hausdorff JM (2007) The sensory feedback mechanisms enabling couples to walk synchronously: an initial investigation. J Neuroeng Rehabil 4(1):1CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Lira Yu
    • 1
  • Yuko Hattori
    • 2
  • Shinya Yamamoto
    • 3
  • Masaki Tomonaga
    • 2
  1. 1.Wildlife Research CenterKyoto UniversityKyotoJapan
  2. 2.Primate Research InstituteKyoto UniversityInuyamaJapan
  3. 3.Institute for Advanced StudyKyoto UniversityKyotoJapan

Personalised recommendations