Advertisement

Getting the Embodied Emotional Survival Strategy

  • Gernot Hauke
Chapter

Abstract

Survival Strategies are cognitive-affective schemata. They describe learned behaviour paths that in the course of life have often led to the desired satisfaction of needs. They are offered as behaviour options by our Impulsive System in certain situations as they have already proved themselves effective on a whole range of occasions. This “offer” happens very quickly and unconsciously, i.e., one doesn’t give it any further thought. It secures the emotional balance in the short term, and preserves a minimum of ability to act and feeling of competence. In condensed form it provides essential information on how a person “ticks” in problematic situations. We are dealing here with a “schematic” answer. Although it often fits and leads to the desired satisfaction of needs, the schema does not take into account the special features of new situations, e.g., when a certain type of interaction partner enters the equation. Then in the long term, even an increased input of energy will not achieve the target. Over time this gives rise to various symptoms: stress and exhaustion, burn-out, withdrawal, physical symptoms etc. soon appear. The Survival Strategy conveys not only a fundamental understanding for the client, but also gives the therapeutic intervention direction, structure and content. For this reason it is worked out as far as possible within the first 3 h of the therapy. Clients generally bring sufficient occasions for this to the session. These tend to be about their everyday, practical problem situations that push them again and again to the limits of their available behaviour repertoire.

References

  1. Barsalou, L. W. (2015). Situated conceptualization: Theory and application. In Y. Coello & M. H. Fischer (Eds.), Perceptual and emotional embodiment: Foundations of embodied cognition. East Sussex: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  2. Bischof, N. (2008). Psychologie. Ein Grundkurs für Anspruchsvolle. Stuttgart: Kohl-hammer.Google Scholar
  3. Deutsch, R. (2017). Associative and propositional processes from the perspective of the Reflective-Impulsive Model. In R. Deutsch, B. Gawronski, & W. Hofmann (Eds.), Reflective and impulsive determinants of human behavior (pp. 51–68). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  4. Gjelsvik, B., Lovric, D., & Williams, M. (2015). Embodied cognition and emotional disorders: Embodiment and abstraction in understanding depression. Psychopathology Review, 1–50.Google Scholar
  5. Hauke, G. (2006). Self-regulation and mindfulness. European Psychotherapy, 6, 19–52.Google Scholar
  6. Hauke, G., Lohr, C., & Pietrzak, T. (2016). Moving the mind: Embodied cognition in Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT). European Psychotherapy, 13, 154–178.Google Scholar
  7. Körner, A., Topolinski, S., & Strack, F. (2015). Routes to embodiment. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 940.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00940CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  8. Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., & Ganis, G. (2006). The case for mental imagery. New York: Oxford Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Miles, L. K., Nind, L. K., & Macrea, C. N. (2010). Moving through time. Psychological Science, 21, 222–223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Pittman, T. S., & Zeigler, K. R. (2006). Basic human needs. In A. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: A handbook of basic principles (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  11. Sulz, S. (1994). Strategische Kurzzeittherapie: Effiziente Wege zur wirksamen Psychotherapie. München: CIP-Medien.Google Scholar
  12. Williams, J. M. G., Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., & Kabat-Zinn, J. (2007). The mindful way through depression: Freeing yourself from chronic unhappiness. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Embodiment Resources Academy (ERA)MunichGermany

Personalised recommendations