Advertisement

The Impact of Speaking a Tone Language on Music Aptitude

  • Niloufar Saraei
Chapter
Part of the English Language Education book series (ELED, volume 16)

Abstract

A rapidly growing body of scientific research suggests that there is a close relationship between music and language in that training in one can influence the other. Recent studies show that the mechanisms involved in processing music and language can improve our understanding of each domain. Nevertheless, most of the studies have merely focused on the positive effects of music on language processing. Despite its significance, evidence for positive effects of language on music seems to be rare. The main purpose of this study is to investigate the correlation between speaking a tone language and musical aptitude. It is hypothesized that speaking a tone language has a positive effect on music perception and that musicians have a better capability in pitch discrimination in comparison to non-musicians. A sample of 40 participants was recruited and included four groups: tone-language speaking musicians/non-musicians, and native speakers of non-tonal languages with/without musical knowledge. Using Gordon’s musical aptitude test, the positive effects of having a tone-language background, as well as having advanced musical knowledge on the perception of musical pitch, were investigated. The results showed that tone-language speaking non-musicians had a better ability in pitch discrimination than the non-musicians who were natives of non-tonal languages. Furthermore, the findings indicated that academically trained musicians outperformed non-tonal language speaking non-musicians in the presented music aptitude test. This leads us to the conclusion that having musical knowledge and a tone-language background can both have positive effects on music perception.

References

  1. Besson, M., & Schön, D. (2001). Comparison between language and music. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 930(1), 232–258.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05736.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bidelman, G. M., Gandour, J. T., & Krishnan, A. (2011a). Cross-domain effects of music and language experience on the representation of pitch in the human auditory brainstem. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 425–434.  https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21362 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bidelman, G. M., Gandour, J. T., & Krishnan, A. (2011b). Musicians and tone-language speakers share enhanced brainstem encoding but not perceptual benefits for musical pitch. Brain and Cognition, 77(1), 1–10.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2011.07.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bidelman, G. M., Hutka, S., & Moreno, S. (2013). Tone language speakers and musicians share enhanced perceptual and cognitive abilities for musical pitch: Evidence for bidirectionality between the domains of language and music. PLoS ONE, 8(4), 1–11.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0060676 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bidelman, G. M., & Krishnan, A. (2010). Effects of reverberation on brainstem representation of speech in musicians and non-musicians. Brain Research, 1355, 112–125.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2010.07.100 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Brown, S. (2000). The “musilanguage” model of music evolution. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The origins of music (pp. 271–300). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  7. Chandrasekaran, B., Krishnan, A., & Gandour, J. T. (2009). Relative influence of musical and linguistic experience on early cortical processing of pitch contours. Brain and Language, 108(1), 1–9.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2008.02.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cooper, A., & Wang, Y. (2012). The influence of linguistic and musical experience on Cantonese word learning. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 131(6), 4756–4769.  https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4714355 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Deutsch, D., Henthorn, T., Marvin, E., & Xu, H. (2006). Absolute pitch among American and Chinese conservatory students: Prevalence differences, and evidence for a speech-related critical period. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 19(2), 719–722.  https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2151799 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Duanmu, S. (2004). Tone and non-tone languages: An alternative to language typology and parameters. Language and Linguistics, 5(4), 891–924.Google Scholar
  11. Fonseca-Mora, C., Toscano-Fuentes, C., & Wermke, K. (2011). Melodies that help: The relation between language aptitude and musical intelligence. Anglistik International Journal of English Studies, 22(1), 101–118.Google Scholar
  12. Gordon, E. E. (1984). Learning sequences in music: A contemporary music learning theory. Chicago: GIA Publications.Google Scholar
  13. Gordon, E. E. (1989). Testing music aptitudes from preschool through college. Gordon Institute for Music Learning Newsletter, 2, 4–6.Google Scholar
  14. Koelsch, S., Gunter, T. C., v Cramon, D. Y., Zysset, S., Lohmann, G., & Friederici, A. D. (2002). Bach speaks: A cortical “language-network” serves the processing of music. NeuroImage, 17(2), 956–966.  https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.115 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Lee, C., & Lee, Y. (2010). Perception of musical pitch and lexical tones by Mandarin-speaking musicians. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 127(1), 481–490.  https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3266683 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. McDermott, J., & Hauser, M. (2005). The origins of music: Innateness, uniqueness, and evolution. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 23(1), 29–59.  https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2005.23.1.29 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Parbery-Clark, A., Skoe, E., & Kraus, N. (2009). Musical experience limits the degradative effects of background noise on the neural processing of sound. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 29(45), 14100–14107.  https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3256-09.2009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Patel, A. D. (2008). Music, language, and the brain. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Slevc, L., Rosenberg, J., & Patel, A. D. (2009). Making psycholinguistics musical: Self-paced reading time evidence for shared processing of linguistic and musical syntax. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(2), 374–381.  https://doi.org/10.3758/16.2.374 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Walters, D. L. (1991). Edwin Gordon’s music aptitude work. The Quarterly Journal of Music Teaching and Learning, 2(1–2), 65–72.Google Scholar
  21. Wong, P. C. M., Skoe, E., Russo, N. M., Dees, T., & Kraus, N. (2007). Musical experience shapes human brainstem encoding of linguistic pitch patterns. Nature Neuroscience, 10(4), 420–422.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1872 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Xu, Y. (1997). Contextual tonal variations in Mandarin. Journal of Phonetics, 25(1), 61–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Niloufar Saraei
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Linguistics and Centre for Teacher Education, Unit for Language Learning and Teaching ResearchUniversity of ViennaViennaAustria

Personalised recommendations