Bird Species pp 95-107 | Cite as
Timing Matters: Allochronic Contributions to Population Divergence
Abstract
Globally, most birds reproduce to some extent seasonally, and the timing of their annual breeding events involves regulation by biological clocks. Biological clocks also regulate diel activities, including song and other courtship behaviors which occur at certain times of day. Differences between individuals in the timing of display and breeding (i.e., allochrony) can drive genetic divergence, contribute to isolation between populations, and ultimately lead to speciation. Isolation by breeding time is thought to be common in areas where reproductive seasons differ greatly over short distances, for example in tropical regions. Here we introduce the ways biological clocks of birds function, and review evidence for variation between individuals and between populations. The potential importance of allochrony for the speciation of birds is underpinned by the periodic growth and regression of their reproductive organs, by their rigid migration programs, and possibly also by their learnt, well-timed mating behaviors (in particular song). We exemplify isolation by time in tropical songbirds and in migratory species. Potentially, further contexts that could promote allochronic isolation in birds include the differentiation between urban and rural populations, as urbanization is commonly associated with modified timing of breeding and song.
Keywords
Isolation by time Biological rhythm Circannual Circadian Seasonality Chronotype Assortative mating Song Tropical Migration6.1 Timing Is Everything!
Diel timing every 24 hours aligns with the rotation of the Earth (line S/N indicates the rotational axis, and the filled circle represents a specific time point of reference) and annual timing aligns with the Earth’s orbit around the Sun every 12 months (shown by the eclipse). These astronomical cycles have favored the evolution of internal circadian (approximately daily) and circannual (approximately annual) clocks. Circadian rhythms regulate the timing of many daily activities, including song and courtship. For annual timing, the ability to respond to changes in day length (photoperiodism) depends on the circadian clock system. Birds utilize both types of internal, innate timing mechanism to regulate reproduction, molt, migration, and other processes [adapted from Helm and Lincoln (2017)]
In this chapter, we argue that biological timekeeping, in particular on an annual and daily time scale, can promote isolation between differently timed individuals and can be a driver of the diversification of birds (Hendry and Day 2005; Taylor and Friesen 2017). We will first introduce basic background on biological rhythms and then demonstrate the diversity of avian timing, highlighting putative isolation mechanisms.
6.2 Clockworks
Because astronomically based environmental rhythms are so predictable (Fig. 6.1), organisms have evolved a range of preparatory strategies for coping with them. A big step early in the evolution of life (Hut and Beersma 2011) was anticipation of environmental changes, when cyanobacteria progressed from simply reacting to periodical changes to predicting them in advance. Since then, organisms have evolved sophisticated behaviors that rely on precise prediction of the periodic changes in the environment. For example, many migratory bird species leave their breeding grounds before they become inhospitable, and initiate their return movements in anticipation of the upcoming spring (Numata and Helm 2014). Likewise, birds are usually not simply woken by morning twilight but will have anticipated the upcoming day well in advance (Foster and Kreitzman 2005).
Circadian regulation of rooster crowing. The figure shows crowing of a rooster (black marks) over 4 weeks. First, the bird was recorded under cycles of 12 hours of light followed by 12 hours of dimmed light at night, indicated by yellow and gray backgrounds, respectively. Then, the bird was recorded under constant conditions of dim light (gray background only). The onset of its crowing became progressively earlier under constant conditions. Time “0” on the x-axis shows start of light; time points “12” mark 12 hours before and after the start of light. Each line in the graph (y-axis) shows a day of recording [adapted from Shimmura and Yoshimura (2013), Shimmura et al. (2015)]
For the circadian clock, the mechanisms that drive this rhythm have been intensively studied, highlighted by the recent award of the 2017 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine. We now know that rhythms are generated within cells by a loop of so-called clock genes, which switch each other on and off to measure out the period of circa 24 hours. Many other genes are involved, for example by linking clocks to metabolism. Then, the millions of cellular clocks in a bird, fly or human need to be coordinated to produce useful body time. This is achieved, for example by nerve-cell coupling in brain centers and by hormones, such as melatonin which peaks at night (Foster and Kreitzman 2005). Through various links and feedback loops, organisms thus achieve coordinated rhythms within their bodies. Because clocks are so important to the life of organisms, they are very sensitive to important cues from the environment, in particular to changes of light and darkness. Birds perceive these changes and adjust (“synchronize”) their biological rhythms so that they match the 24-hour daily cycle of the environment (Fig. 6.2).
Innate, internal cycles of testis growth and regression in male Eurasian Blackcaps Sylvia atricapilla. Blackcaps on the Cape Verde islands go through two reproductive cycles per year, whereas European Blackcaps go through one annual cycle. In captivity, the birds show the time patterns of their native populations, and hybrids are intermediate. Male Blackcaps were kept under the photoperiodic condition of the Cape Verde islands. (a) Number of daylight hours. (b) Testicular cycles of Blackcaps from Cape Verde (red), southern Germany (blue), and hybrids (purple) of the two populations maintained under identical conditions [data from Berthold and Querner (1993), figure adapted from Goymann and Helm (2014)]
As described above for circadian rhythms, circannual cycles also typically continue if a bird is prevented from witnessing environmental changes (Gwinner 1996; Goymann and Helm 2014). For example, in stonechats (songbirds within the genus Saxicola), the growth and reduction of reproductive organs is driven by a circannual clock. When pairs were kept in aviaries under constant day length of 12 hours of light alternating with 12 hours of night, males and females reproduced if they came into reproductive condition at similar times (Gwinner 1996; Goymann and Helm 2014). Their sons and daughters never experienced rhythms in their environment, but nonetheless showed annual cycles of breeding condition and molt, driven by their circannual clocks. Under these conditions, breeding condition of the stonechats drifted from the annual cycle of 365 days, much like the crowing behavior of the rooster drifted from the 24-hour cycle shown in Fig. 6.2. Under natural conditions, circannual rhythms like circadian rhythms synchronize to environmental cycles (Gwinner 1986; Helm and Lincoln 2017). The most important synchronizing cue on an annual time scale is photoperiod (day length), although other cues can also play a role (Helm et al. 2006; Goymann and Helm 2014).
The precise timing of behavior and activities of birds under natural conditions is influenced by many additional factors that modify the outcome of biological timekeeping. For example, the time of song and breeding can be influenced by age, experience, dominance, body condition, and various local environmental factors (Poesel et al. 2006; Helm et al. 2006; Shimmura et al. 2015). But importantly for the subject of avian speciation, innate biological rhythms provide a powerful substrate for evolutionary change (Fig. 6.3; Helm and Visser 2010; Taylor and Friesen 2017).
6.3 Allochrony: Differences in Timing Between Individuals, Populations, and Species
Chronotype of individual songbirds schematically depicted against the backdrop of a Scottish forest woodland. The image shows three individuals during early morning hours which differ slightly from each other in their biological clock times (depicted by a clock imposed on each bird’s head). For two of these individuals, subjectively, the time for starting dawn song (set to just after 03:00 hours in May in Scotland) is yet to come. The third individual, in contrast, has a slightly earlier biological rhythm. For this bird, the time for initiating song has already started (depicted by its biological clock showing 03:05 hours), and the bird is accordingly singing. Original artwork: Robyn Womack
Timing differences between individuals can have important reproductive consequences. Courtship and reproductive behaviors are often fitted to short phases of conducive environmental conditions. Therefore, reproductive success can differ between individuals performing these behaviors early or late in the day or year (Hau et al. 2017). For example, in Fig. 6.4, the bird whose clock is the most advanced is already singing, while the others are still preparing for the day. Several studies have shown that a male that starts dawn song particularly early in the day has high chances of enjoying increased reproductive success (Poesel et al. 2006; Hau et al. 2017). A recent review article (Hau et al. 2017) discusses to which extent the timing of behaviors may therefore be under sexual selection. As detailed by Hau and colleagues, for the timing of behaviors such as song to be a candidate for sexual selection, it would need to increase matings but also impose a cost on the singing bird (e.g., shortened sleep duration). For sexual selection to take effect, there also needs to be a genetic contribution to the specific timing of an individual, which can then be modified by evolution (Helm and Visser 2010). Clock genes, and potentially their epigenetic modification, have been shown to play a role in such modifications in a number of taxa (Caprioli et al. 2012; Hau et al. 2017; Taylor and Friesen 2017; Saino et al. 2017).
However, sexual selection is not the only factor that could drive and maintain genetic variation in the timing of individuals. It is very likely that there are also other pressures, such as natural selection for greater fecundity or survival (Hau et al. 2017). For example, for both males and females, waking early could enable improved feeding opportunities, and an early return from migration could provide access to superior breeding territories (Bearhop et al. 2005; Hau et al. 2017). Thereby, individuals which due to a mutation rise or return earlier could enjoy greater fecundity or survival. Maintaining diversity in timing however requires that such benefits are not universal. For example, early risers may sometimes suffer energetic costs on brisk mornings or predation by nocturnal predators, counterbalancing natural selection for earlier onset of activity. Likewise, early-returning migrants can get caught in severe weather in late winter, from which late-returning migrants are spared (Newton 2008). A role of biological clocks in the contexts of sexual and natural selection has been demonstrated in a range of organisms, especially in insects and fish (Kaiser et al. 2016; Taylor and Friesen 2017).
For variation between individuals to drive divergence within a population, there must be isolating mechanisms. For example, because reproductive behaviors often occur during short phases only, males and females that are similarly timed might preferentially mate with each other (Fig. 6.4). Such “assortative mating” could then accelerate genetic changes in the clockworks, further driving divergence within a population (Bearhop et al. 2005; Dominoni et al. 2013a, b). An example is once again Blackcaps (Bearhop et al. 2005). In this species, some individuals began making a genetically determined shorter migration journey and returned to the breeding grounds earlier in the spring (Newton 2008). Males and females carrying out this shorter migration preferentially mated with each other soon after arrival on the breeding grounds, before conspecifics taking a longer route arrived (Bearhop et al. 2005). This difference in mating times could have contributed to the observed, fast spread of the novel migration behavior.
Variation in chronotype within populations connects seamlessly to differences between populations, if time-structuring factors isolate parts of a population from others. Migration, as described above, can be one such factor. Others can be important ecological factors that differ in timing. Here, the most convincing examples come from insects, for example, from fruit flies that have specialized on host plants that differ in timing of fruition (Taylor and Friesen 2017). In birds, most examples of allochronic divergence involve some separation in space at the time of breeding. Spatial distances do not need to be large because habitats can differ in optimal timing even over short distances, for example in tropical environments. Thus, breeding timing differs dramatically between populations of Rufous-collared Sparrows Zonotrichia capensis that breed on the western and eastern slopes of the Ecuadorian Andes, which in turn differ in the timing of rainy seasons (Moore et al. 2005). These populations do not overlap in the timing of breeding and have already started to diverge (see below).
On larger spatial scales, populations may differ systematically in reproductive timing, for example across latitudinal gradients, where more northerly breeding populations typically reproduce later in the year (Helm 2009). One extreme example is stonechat populations from different geographic origins that differ so starkly in reproductive timing (Helm 2009) that across the distribution range, some local populations will be in breeding condition at any time of the year. Another example is the blackcaps shown in Fig. 6.3. Blackcaps on the Cape Verde islands go through two full annual cycles of gonadal growth and regression and breed during two distinct breeding seasons, unlike Eurasian Blackcaps that have a single gonadal cycle and breeding season per year. Figure 6.3 shows that under identical captivity conditions, these differences were retained: Each population showed its characteristic reproductive cycle. Moreover, the differences were heritable, as indicated by the intermediate patterns in hybrids (Berthold and Querner 1993; Goymann and Helm 2014).
With greater taxonomic distance, such timing differences generally become even more evident, and links to underlying biological clocks are starting to be revealed (Taylor and Friesen 2017). For example, differences in breeding time have been associated with latitudinal clines in clock gene variants, with the most convincing examples relating to fish and insects (Hut et al. 2013; Taylor and Friesen 2017).
6.4 Isolation by the Clock
If differences in timing are underwritten by differences in the birds’ biological rhythms, as in the stonechats and Blackcaps, they can become powerful isolation mechanisms and ultimately lead to speciation. For example, stonechats from differently timed populations mentioned above were crossbred with each other under identical captive conditions (Helm 2009). Like the Blackcaps shown in Fig. 6.3, the captive stonechats displayed the typical timing of their populations. Only when the breeding phases of the two populations overlapped were pairs from different populations successful in producing viable offspring (Helm 2009). Thus in the wild, such allochronic populations might already be limited in possible gene flow if they were to meet, for example through dispersing individuals.
In contrast to the well-established concept of “isolation by space,” such “isolation by time” (Hendry and Day 2005) has only more recently been established as a formal idea. In wild birds, some convincing examples for a role of allochrony for speciation come from seabirds that nest colonially on islands (Taylor and Friesen 2017), for example Band-rumped Storm Petrel Oceanodroma castro (Friesen et al. 2007). On some islands, different parts of the Band-rumped Storm Petrel population breed sympatrically, but at completely different times of the year, and thereafter leave the colonies for the rest of the year (Friesen et al. 2007). As a consequence, these allochronically breeding petrels never meet, and the island population shows substantial genetic substructure by reproductive timing. In this example, breeders on one and the same island are in effect allopatric during reproductive seasons and have greatly diverged from each other.
Initial isolation of allochronically breeding populations has also been shown for wild birds that reproduce in neighboring locations. The Andean Rufous-collared Sparrows described above have diverged genetically, but furthermore, they show also signs of divergence in sexual signals. When song from the allochronic populations of the opposite Andean slope was played back to the local population, the receiving birds responded more hesitantly than when they heard songs from synchronic populations (Danner et al. 2011). This might suggest that avian song, which is only performed during the breeding season, may diverge quickly between populations that never hear each other sing, and thereby further accelerate allochronic isolation (Wilkins et al. 2013). Allochronic divergence of birds over short distances might be relatively common in tropical regions, where seasonality is often very local. A recent study involving 57 New World bird species related speciation to allochrony in response to different seasonal timing of food availability and ultimately precipitation (Quintero et al. 2014). Findings of comparative analyses were seen to support a so-called asynchrony of seasons hypothesis (Martin et al. 2009), which proposes that allochrony is an important contributor to the high speciation rates in the tropics.
6.5 Conclusions
Most studies on divergence between populations and on speciation have focused on spatial segregation, but recent evidence emphasizes also a role for temporal segregation. The evolutionary mechanisms are probably somewhat different because unlike spatial behavior, temporal behavior is based on innate, internal biological mechanisms. Because biological rhythms differ somewhat between individuals, they offer rich substrate for evolutionary change and can contribute to isolation and speciation, for example by mating time. The potential importance of allochrony for avian speciation might be increased by seasonally displayed mating behaviors, in particular by song, which may function as an additional isolation factor (Wilkins et al. 2013). Examples for allochronic divergence of bird populations that breed in relative proximity, so far, stem mainly from migratory species and from tropical areas. However, allochronic isolation might be more widespread and could occur, so to speak, at our door step. For example, birds breeding in urban habitats often sing much earlier in the day and reproduce far earlier in the year than conspecifics in nearby rural areas. Such behavioral differences on both time scales have been linked to the birds’ biological rhythms (Dominoni et al. 2013a, b) and could contribute to decreasing gene exchange between urban and rural populations (Gill and Brumm 2013).
Further Reading
- Hau M, Dominoni D, Casagrande S, Buck CL, Wagner G, Hazlerigg D et al (2017) Timing as a sexually selected trait: the right mate at the right moment. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 372CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Moore IT, Bonier F, Wingfield JC (2005) Reproductive asynchrony and population divergence between two tropical bird populations. Behav Ecol 16:755–762CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Numata H, Helm B (2014) Annual, lunar and tidal clocks: patterns and mechanisms of nature’s enigmatic rhythms. Springer Japan, TokyoCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Taylor RS, Friesen VL (2017) The role of allochrony in speciation. Mol Ecol 26:3330–3342CrossRefGoogle Scholar
References
- Ashley NT, Ubuka T, Schwabl I, Goymann W, Salli BM, Bentley GE et al (2014) Revealing a circadian clock in captive arctic-breeding songbirds, lapland longspurs (Calcarius lapponicus), under constant illumination. J Biol Rhythm 29:456–469CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Battley PF (2006) Consistent annual schedules in a migratory shorebird. Biol Lett 2:517–520CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bearhop S, Fiedler W, Furness RW, Votier SC, Waldron S, Newton J et al (2005) Assortative mating as a mechanism for rapid evolution of a migratory divide. Science 310:502–504CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Berthold P, Querner U (1993) Genetic and photoperiodic control of an avian reproductive cycle. Cell Mol Life Sci 49(4):342CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Caprioli M, Ambrosini R, Boncoraglio G, Gatti E, Romano A, Romano M et al (2012) Clock gene variation is associated with breeding phenology and maybe under directional selection in the migratory Barn Swallow. PLoS One 7:e35140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Danner JE, Danner RM, Bonier F, Martin PR, Small TW, Moore IT (2011) Female, but not male, tropical sparrows respond more strongly to the local song dialect: implications for population divergence. Am Nat 178:53–63CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Dominoni D, Quetting M, Partecke J (2013a) Artificial light at night advances avian reproductive physiology. Proc Biol Sci 280:20123017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Dominoni DM, Helm B, Lehmann M, Dowse HB, Partecke J (2013b) Clocks for the city: circadian differences between forest and city songbirds. Proc Biol Sci 280:20130593CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Foster RG, Kreitzman L (2005) In rhythms of life: the biological clocks that control the daily lives of every living thing. Yale University Press, New Haven, CTGoogle Scholar
- Friesen VL, Smith AL, Gómez-Díaz E, Bolton M, Furness RW, González-Solís J et al (2007) Sympatric speciation by allochrony in a seabird. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104:18589–18594CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Gill D, Brumm H (2013) Avian urban ecology: behavioural and physiological adaptations. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Goymann W, Helm B (2014) Seasonality of life-cycles in tropical birds – circannual rhythms and Zeitgeber. In: Numata H, Helm B (eds) Annual, Lunar and tidal clocks: patterns and mechanisms of nature’s enigmatic rhythms. Springer Japan, Tokyo, pp 237–275Google Scholar
- Gwinner E (1986) Circannual clocks. Springer, BerlinGoogle Scholar
- Gwinner E (1996) Circannual clocks in avian reproduction and migration. Ibis 138:47–63CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hau M, Dominoni D, Casagrande S, Buck CL, Wagner G, Hazlerigg D et al (2017) Timing as a sexually selected trait: the right mate at the right moment. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 372:20160249CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Helm B (2009) Geographically distinct reproductive schedules in a changing world: costly implications in captive Stonechats. Integr Comp Biol 49:563–579CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Helm B, Lincoln GA (2017) Circannual rhythms anticipate the earth’s annual periodicity. In: Kumar V (ed) Biological timekeeping: clocks, rhythms and behaviour. Springer India, New Delhi, pp 545–569CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Helm B, Visser ME (2010) Heritable circadian period length in a wild bird population. Proc B Biol Sci 277:3335–3342CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Helm B, Piersma T, Van der Jeugd H (2006) Sociable schedules: interplay between avian seasonal and social behaviour. Anim Behav 72:245–262; 1215Google Scholar
- Helm B, Visser ME, Schwartz W, Kronfeld-Schor N, Gerkema M, Piersma T et al (2017) Two sides of a coin: ecological and chronobiological perspectives of timing in the wild. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 372:20160246CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hendry AP, Day T (2005) Population structure attributable to reproductive time: isolation by time and adaptation by time. Mol Ecol 14:901–916CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hut RA, Beersma DGM (2011) Evolution of time-keeping mechanisms: early emergence and adaptation to photoperiod. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 366:2141–2154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hut RA, Paolucci S, Dor R, Kyriacou CP, Daan S (2013) Latitudinal clines: an evolutionary view on biological rhythms. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 280:20130433CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kaiser TS, Poehn B, Szkiba D, Preussner M, Sedlazeck FJ, Zrim A et al (2016) The genomic basis of circadian and circalunar timing adaptations in a midge. Nature 540:69–73CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Martin PR, Bonier F, Moore IT, Tewksbury JJ (2009) Latitudinal variation in the asynchrony of seasons: implications for higher rates of population differentiation and speciation in the tropics. Ideas Ecol Evol 2Google Scholar
- Moore IT, Bonier F, Wingfield JC (2005) Reproductive asynchrony and population divergence between two tropical bird populations. Behav Ecol 16:755–762CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Newton I (2008) The migration ecology of birds. Academic, LondonGoogle Scholar
- Numata H, Helm B (2014) Annual, lunar and tidal clocks: patterns and mechanisms of nature’s enigmatic rhythms. Springer Japan, TokyoCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Nussey DH, Postma E, Gienapp P, Visser ME (2005) Selection on heritable phenotypic plasticity in a wild bird population. Science 310:304–306CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Poesel A, Kunc HP, Foerster K, Johnsen A, Kempenaers B (2006) Early birds are sexy: male age, dawn song and extrapair paternity in blue tits, Cyanistes (formerly Parus) caeruleus. Anim Behav 72:531–538CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Quintero I, González-Caro S, Zalamea P-C, Cadena CD (2014) Asynchrony of seasons: genetic differentiation associated with geographic variation in climatic seasonality and reproductive phenology. Am Nat 184:352–363CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Saino N, Ambrosini R, Albetti B, Caprioli M, De Giorgio B, Gatti E et al (2017) Migration phenology and breeding success are predicted by methylation of a photoperiodic gene in the barn swallow. Sci Rep 7:45412CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Shimmura T, Yoshimura T (2013) Circadian clock determines the timing of rooster crowing. Curr Biol 23:R231–R2R3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Shimmura T, Ohashi S, Yoshimura T (2015) The highest-ranking rooster has priority to announce the break of dawn. Sci Rep 5:11683CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Taylor RS, Friesen VL (2017) The role of allochrony in speciation. Mol Ecol 26:3330–3342CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wilkins MR, Seddon N, Safran RJ (2013) Evolutionary divergence in acoustic signals: causes and consequences. Trends Ecol Evol 28:156–166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2012.10.002 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Williams TD (2012) Physiological adaptations for breeding in birds. Princeton University Press, PrincetonCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Zantke J, Ishikawa-Fujiwara T, Arboleda E, Lohs C, Schipany K, Hallay N et al (2013) Circadian and circalunar clock interactions in a marine annelid. Cell Rep 5:99–113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Zhang L, Hastings MH, Green EW, Tauber E, Sladek M, Webster SG et al (2013) Dissociation of circadian and circatidal timekeeping in the marine crustacean Eurydice pulchra. Curr Biol 23:1863–1873CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Copyright information
This chapter is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits use, duplication, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, a link is provided to the Creative Commons license and any changes made are indicated.The images or other third party material in this chapter are included in the work's Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in the credit line; if such material is not included in the work's Creative Commons license and the respective action is not permitted by statutory regulation, users will need to obtain permission from the license holder to duplicate, adapt or reproduce the material.