Advertisement

Moving Bodies

  • Brigid M. Costello
Chapter
Part of the Springer Series on Cultural Computing book series (SSCC)

Abstract

We are used to thinking and speaking about knowledge as something primarily produced by abstract thought, despite being aware that knowledge also resides in our practices, in our doing as well as our thinking. Practical bodily knowledge and its relationship to rhythm is the focus of this chapter. We explore the bodily intelligence involved in rhythmic experience, its relationship to the ways we can move our bodies and the ways these bodies can be in turn moved by it. As we will see, this type of bodily intelligence communicates but does so in a different way to words. It speaks of anticipation, expectation, speed and temporal relationships. It speaks of flux and flow, and patterned durations. Our interaction designs can both speak this language to our users and listen to what their rhythmed bodies are saying. To evaluate these aspects of our designs we need to choose methods that tap into our own bodily intelligence as well as that of our participants. We also need to attend to the impact our interface rhythms might have on the human body as it moves. Leading us into this discussion is an interview with Simon Barker, a composer, drummer and lecturer in Jazz studies who has spent several years working with traditional Korean musicians and shamans. Not surprisingly, given his practice, Simon Barker’s interview focuses on the relationship between rhythm and bodily movement: a relationship that he says is integral to understanding rhythm and the knowledge it produces. The three sections that follow Simon Barker’s interview expand on the theme of bodily movement to discuss: what kinds of knowledge are involved in rhythmic movement? How can this type of rhythmic knowledge be communicated and analysed? And what are some of the factors designers need to consider when designing rhythms for moving bodies?

References

  1. Butler BE, Trainor LJ (2015) The musician redefined: a behavioral assessment of rhythm perception in professional club DJs. Timing & Time Percept 3(1–2):116–132.  https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-03002041CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Carter M, Velloso E, Downs J, Sellen A, O’Hara K, Vetere F (2016) PathSync: multi-user gestural interaction with touchless rhythmic Path Mimicry. Paper presented at the proceedings of the 2016 CHI conference on human factors in computing systems, Santa Clara, California, USAGoogle Scholar
  3. Carvalho D, Bessa M, Magalhaes L, Carrapatoso E (2016) Age group differences in performance using diverse input modalities: insertion task evaluation. Paper presented at the proceedings of the XVII international conference on human computer interaction, Salamanca, SpainGoogle Scholar
  4. Catterson P (2009) I promised myself I would never let it leave my body’s memory. Dance Res J 41(2):3–11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Costello BM (2016) The rhythm of game interactions: player experience and rhythm in Minecraft and Don’t Starve. Games and Cult.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1555412016646668CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Costello B, Muller L, Amitani S, Edmonds E (2005) Understanding the experience of interactive art: Iamascope in Beta_space. In: Interactive entertainment 2005, University of Technology Sydney, Australia, 23–25 November 2005. Creativity & Cognition Studios Press, pp 49–56Google Scholar
  7. Eshun K (n.d.) Abducted by audio. Cybernetic Culture Research Unit. http://www.ccru.net/swarm3/3_abducted.htm. Accessed March 2013
  8. Eshun K (1998) More brilliant than the sun: adventures in sonic fiction. Quartet, London, UKGoogle Scholar
  9. Gillett N (2016) The month in games: virtual reality falters, while Mario makes a play for the smartphone market. The Guardian, UK, 24 Sept 2016Google Scholar
  10. Hasty CF (1997) Meter as rhythm. Oxford University Press, New York, USAGoogle Scholar
  11. Iyer V (2002) Embodied mind, situated cognition, and expressive microtiming in African-American music. Music Percept 19(3):387–414CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Jellayrei (2016) FYI about overwatch FoV and motion sickness caused by FPS’s. Overwatch forum, vol 26 (May 2016). Reddit.com. Online: https://www.reddit.com/r/Overwatch/comments/4l79ti/fyi_about_overwatch_fov_and_motion_sickness/
  13. Johnson M (2007) The meaning of the body: aesthetics of human understanding. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  14. Myers N (2012) Dance your PhD: embodied animations, body experiments, and the affective entanglements of life science research. Body & Soc 18(1):151–189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Newman J (2002) In search of the videogame player. New Media & Soc 4(3):405–421MathSciNetCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Nintendo (2016) Metroid Prime: Federation Force. Nintendo 3DS. Nintendo, JapanGoogle Scholar
  17. Olsen A, McHose C (2014) The place of dance: a somatic guide to dancing and dance-making. Wesleyan University Press, USAGoogle Scholar
  18. Phillips-Silver J, Trainor LJ (2007) Hearing what the body feels: auditory encoding of rhythmic movement. Cognition 105(3):533–546.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.11.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Playdead (2010) Limbo. Video game. PC, PlayStation, Xbox, IOS, Android. Playdead, Denmark; Microsoft Game Studios, USAGoogle Scholar
  20. Rainer Y (2009) Trio A: genealogy, documentation, notation. Dance Res J 41(2):12–18CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Roholt TC (2014) Groove: a phenomenology of rhythmic Nuance. Bloomsbury, New York, USAGoogle Scholar
  22. Sklar D (1994) Can bodylore be brought to its senses? J Am Folklore 107(423):9–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of the Arts and MediaThe University of New South WalesSydneyAustralia

Personalised recommendations