Advertisement

Empirical Study of Physiological Characteristics Accompanied by Tactile Thermal Perception

Relationship Between Changes in Thermal Gradients and Skin Conductance Responses
  • Takafumi ShinodaEmail author
  • Kouki Shimomura
  • Kentaro Kotani
  • Satoshi Suzuki
  • Takafumi Asao
  • Shigeyoshi Iizuka
Conference paper
Part of the Lecture Notes in Computer Science book series (LNCS, volume 9734)

Abstract

This paper presents empirical results regarding emotional changes represented using different thermal gradients by measuring skin conductance responses (SCRs), when providing thermal information with various gradients. Participants attached a probe in their right forearm for thermal stimuli, and SCR measurements were conducted when participants perceived the time to detect a temperature change. The SCR amplitude showed a significant tendency to vary between +0.5 and −0.5 [°C/s] and between +0.5 and −0.3 [°C/s] of thermal gradients. The results of this study showed that differences in clear thermal gradients, such as the comparison between warm and cold stimuli, affected emotion, and differences in detailed thermal gradients, such as the comparison between 0.3 and 0.5 [°C/s] of thermal gradients, did not affect the changes in emotion.

Keywords

Emotion Skin conductance responses Thermal gradients Tactile sensation 

Notes

Acknowledgements

Part of the present study was funded by Advanced-biomedical sensing technology research group Kansai University, and Kakenhi of the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (26560036).

References

  1. 1.
    Fukushima, S.: Emotion interface in the fields of entertainment and communication. J. Robot. Soc. Jpn 32(8), 692–695 (2014)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. 2.
    Fukushima, S., et al.: Emotional vibration: enhancement of emotion using a combination of sound and skin sensation to the Pinna. Virtual Real. Soc. Jpn 19(4), 467–476 (2014)MathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  3. 3.
    Akiyama, S., et al.: ThermOn − a novel thermo-musical interface for an enhanced emotional experience. Inf. Process. Soc. Jpn Interact. 2013, 356–360 (2013)Google Scholar
  4. 4.
    Imae, Y., et al: Characteristics of apparent motion induction by tactile stimuli using air-jet. IEICE Technical report, The Institute Electronics Information and Communication Engineers, pp. 53–56, 2007–2010Google Scholar
  5. 5.
    Doi, K., et al.: Sensory characteristics of temperature and its discrimination in the human palm. Int. J. Affect. Eng. 11(3), 419–425 (2012)MathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Nishimura, T., et al.: Relationship between press force and sensory characteristics of temperature in Forefinger. Int. J. Affect. Eng. 13(3), 433–439 (2014)Google Scholar
  7. 7.
    Sato, K.: High-response thermal display unit using spatially distributed warm and cold stimuli. Inf. Process. Soc. Jpn Interact. 923–928 (2012)Google Scholar
  8. 8.
    Mizuno, T., et al: Evaluation of emotional stress in mental workload. IEICE Technical report, The Institute Electronics Information and Communication Engineers, pp. 143–147 (2007)Google Scholar
  9. 9.
    Yamashiro, D., et al.: Sympathetic skin response and emotional changes visual stimuli. No To Hattatsu 36, 372–377 (2004)Google Scholar
  10. 10.
    Ozawa, S.: Standard Textbook. pp. 218–220, 222–223, 386–387. IGAKU-SHOIN, Tokyo (2014)Google Scholar
  11. 11.
    Numata, K., et al.: Electrodermal conditioning: its significance and research trends. J. Literary Assoc. Kwansei Gakuin Univ. 61(2), 55–88 (2001)Google Scholar
  12. 12.
    Miyata, Y.: New Physiological Psychology. vol. 1, pp. 27–30, 210–221. Kitaooji Bookstore (1998)Google Scholar
  13. 13.
    Ikeura, R., et al.: Study on emotional evaluation of robot motions based on galvanic skin reflex. Japan. J. Ergon. 31(5), 355–358 (1995)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. 14.
    Kenshalo, D.R.: Warm and cool thresholds as a function of rate of stimulus temperature change. Percept. Psychophys. 3(2), 81–84 (1968)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. 15.
    Abe, M., et al.: Trial of the measurement of the thermal sensitivity in the oral mucosa and lips and lips neighborhood of the young fellow. Japan. J. Sens. Eval. 16(1), 43–50 (2012)Google Scholar
  16. 16.
    Harrison, J.L., Davis, K.D.: Cold-evoked pain varies with skin type and cooling rate: a psychophysical study in humans. Pain 83(2), 123–135 (1999)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. 17.
    Hata, T.: A trial for quantitative analysis of electrodermal activity by a “lie-detector” electrical kit. Hamamatsu Univ. Sch. Med. Bull. 22, 35–41 (2008)Google Scholar
  18. 18.
    Hirota, A., et al.: A new index for psychophysiological detection of deception: applicability of normalized pulse volume. Japan. J. Physiol. Psychol. Psychophysiol. 21(3), 217–230 (2008)MathSciNetCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. 19.
    Baba, et al.: Development and investigation of a video game interaction that offers termal sensation to users. IPSJ SIG Technical reports, pp. 1–6 (2010)Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Takafumi Shinoda
    • 1
    Email author
  • Kouki Shimomura
    • 1
  • Kentaro Kotani
    • 1
  • Satoshi Suzuki
    • 1
  • Takafumi Asao
    • 1
  • Shigeyoshi Iizuka
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of Mechanical EngineeringKansai UniversityOsakaJapan
  2. 2.Faculty of Business AdministrationKanagawa UniversityKanagawaJapan

Personalised recommendations