Advertisement

Single Trial Variability of Event-Related Brain Potentials as an Index of Neural Efficiency During Working Memory

  • David W. ShucardEmail author
  • Thomas J. Covey
  • Janet L. Shucard
Conference paper
Part of the Lecture Notes in Computer Science book series (LNCS, volume 9743)

Abstract

Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) to a particular stimulus are extracted from the continuous electroencephalogram (EEG) through signal averaging techniques. The most extensively studied ERP component, P300 (P3, or P3b), occurs at approximately 300–800 ms post-stimulus. P3 amplitude and latency are markers of the attentional/cognitive resources devoted to the task and the timing (within msecs) of central processing speed of an individual’s cognitive response to a stimulus, respectively. Variability in the timing and amplitude of components in the single EEG trials that contribute to the averaged ERP has been of particular interest to our laboratory because it may provide an index of central information processing efficiency. Examination of single trial variability (STV) can provide a level of analysis beyond traditional ERP measures and offers a unique marker of the functional integrity of neural pathways. In the present study we examined ERP STV as it relates to WM demand or load during a visual n-back task in normal adult participants. Performance measures and the scalp-recorded EEG were obtained during the n-back task. Frontal and parietal scalp averaged ERPs were derived from the EEG time-locked to the stimuli. The mean and variability (SD) of the peak amplitude and latency were then obtained from the single trial data for each participant and condition. Results showed that as WM load increased, behavioral measures of processing speed slowed, behavioral efficiency decreased, and the number of correct responses decreased. Correlations for both latency and amplitude between the P3 component derived from the averaged ERP and the P3 component derived from the single trials were generally high, indicating that the P3s for individual trials identified by our STV procedure were representative of the P3s in the averaged ERP obtained by standard signal averaging procedures.P3 STV analyses also showed differential effects between frontal and parietal scalp sites for both amplitude and latency variability that were related to WM load. Both frontal P3 latency STV and amplitude increased as WM load increased, indicating decreased neural efficiency associated with an increase in WM load. Single trial ERP variability measures may provide potential physiological markers of the neural efficiency of brain processes engaged in cognitive functions, such as working memory.

Keywords

Working memory ERP Single trial variability 

Notes

Acknowledgements

Partial support for this research was provided by a grant from the National Multiple Sclerosis Society and the Jog for the Jake Foundation.

References

  1. 1.
    Polich, J.: Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 2128–2148 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. 2.
    Kok, A.: On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. Psychophysiology 38, 557–577 (2001)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. 3.
    Baddeley, A.: Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 829–839 (2003)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. 4.
    Carlson, S., Martinkauppi, S., Rama, S., Salli, P., Korvenoja, E., Aronen, H.J.: Distribution of cortical activation during visuospatial n-back tasks as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Cereb. Cortex 8(8), 743–752 (1998)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    Cohen, J.D., Braver, W.M., Nystrom, T.S., Noll, L.E., Jonides, D.C., Smith, J.: Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task. Nature 386, 604–607 (1997)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Fuster, J.M.: Frontal lobes. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 3, 160–165 (1993)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. 7.
    Gathercole, S.E.: Neuropsychology and working memory. Neuropsychology 8, 494–505 (1994)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    Druzgal, T.J., D’Esposito, M.: Dissecting contributions of prefrontal cortex and fusiform face area to face working memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 771–784 (2003)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    Shucard, J.L., Lee, W.H., Safford, A., Shucard, D.W.: The relationship between processing speed and working memory demand in systemic lupus erythematosus: evidence from a visual n-back task. Neuropsychology 24, 45–52 (2011)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    McEvoy, L.K., Smith, M.E., Gevins, A.: Dynamic cortical networks of verbal and spatial working memory: effects of memory load and task practice. Cereb. Cortex 8, 563–574 (1998)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Watter, S., Geffen, G.M., Geffen, L.B.: The n-back as a duel-talk: P300 morphology under divided attention. Psychophysiology 46, 307–311 (2001)Google Scholar
  12. 12.
    Wintink, A.J., Segalowitz, S.J., Cudmore, L.J.: Task complexity and habituation effects on frontal P300 topography. Brain Cogn. 46, 307–311 (2001)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Shucard, J.L., Tekok-Kilic, A., Shiels, K., Shucard, D.W.: Stage and load effects on ERP topography during verbal and spatial working memory. Brain Res. 1254, 49–62 (2009)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. 14.
    Bielak, A.A.M., Hultsch, D.F., Strauss, E., McDonald, S.W.S., Hunter, M.A.: Intraindividual variability in reaction time predicts cognitive outcomes five years later. Neuropsychology 24, 731–741 (2010)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. 15.
    Mohr, P.N.C., Nagel, I.E.: Variability in brain activity as an individual difference measure in neuroscience. J. Neurosci. 30, 7755–7757 (2010)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. 16.
    Specht, C.M., Shucard, D.W.: Single-trial latency variability does not contribute to fast habituation of the long-latency averaged auditory evoked potential in the albino rat. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 100, 462–471 (1996)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. 17.
    Shucard, D.W., Santa Maria, M.P., Specht, M.C., Podkulski, M.: Single-trial latency variability of auditory evoked potentials may indicate immediate memory in the albino rat. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 47, 229–241 (2003)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. 18.
    Usal, A., Segalowitz, S.J.: Sources of P300 attenuation after head injury: single trial amplitude, latency jitter, and EEG power. Psychophysiology 32, 249–256 (1995)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. 19.
    Delorme, A., Makeig, S., Fabre-Thorpe, M., Sejnowski, T.: From single trial EEG to brain area dynamics. Neurocomputing 44–46, 1057–1064 (2002)CrossRefzbMATHGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • David W. Shucard
    • 1
    Email author
  • Thomas J. Covey
    • 1
  • Janet L. Shucard
    • 1
  1. 1.Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurosciences, Department of Neurology, Neuroscience Program, The Jacobs School of Medicine and Biomedical SciencesUniversity at Buffalo, The State University of New YorkBuffaloUSA

Personalised recommendations