Advertisement

Physical and Virtual Manipulatives: What Is “Concrete”?

  • Julie SaramaEmail author
  • Douglas H. Clements
Part of the Mathematics Education in the Digital Era book series (MEDE, volume 7)

Abstract

We discuss research on both physical manipulatives and virtual manipulatives to provide a framework for understanding, creating, implementing, and evaluating efficacious manipulatives—physical, virtual, and a combination of these two. We provide a theoretical framework and a discussion of empirical evidence supporting that framework, for the use of manipulatives in learning and teaching mathematics, from early childhood through the elementary years. From this reformulation, we re-consider the role virtual manipulatives may play in helping students learn mathematics. We conclude that manipulatives are meaningful for learning only with respect to learners’ activities and thinking and that both physical and virtual manipulatives can be useful. When used in comprehensive, well planned, instructional settings, both physical and virtual manipulatives can encourage students to make their knowledge explicit, which helps them build Integrated-Concrete knowledge.

Keywords

Mathematical Idea Number Word Direct Manipulation Algebraic Thinking Concrete Understanding 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Notes

Acknowledgments

This paper was based upon work supported in small part by the Institute of Educational Sciences under Grant No. R305K05157, “Scaling Up TRIAD: Teaching Early Mathematics for Understanding with Trajectories and Technologies”; and Grant No. R305A120813, “Evaluating the Efficacy of Learning Trajectories in Early Mathematics,” and in part by the National Science Foundation under Grant No. DRL-1313695, “Using Rule Space and Poset-based Adaptive Testing Methodologies to Identify Ability Patterns in Early Mathematics and Create a Comprehensive Mathematics Ability Test”; and Grant No. DRL-1118745, “Early Childhood Education in the Context of Mathematics, Science, and Literacy.” Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the funding agencies.

References

  1. Anderson, J. R. (Ed.). (1993). Rules of the mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  2. Ball, D. L. (1992). Magical hopes: Manipulatives and the reform of math education. American Educator, 16(2), 14, 16–18, 46–47.Google Scholar
  3. Bana, J., & Nelson, D. (1978). Distractors in nonverbal mathematics problems. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 9, 55–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barendregt, W., Lindström, B., Rietz-Leppänen, E., Holgersson, I., & Ottosson, T. (2012). Development and evaluation of Fingu: A mathematics iPad game using multi-touch interaction. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the 11th International Conference on Interaction Design and Children, Bremen, Germany.Google Scholar
  5. Baroody, A. J. (1989). Manipulatives don’t come with guarantees. Arithmetic Teacher, 37(2), 4–5.Google Scholar
  6. Baroody, A. J. (1990). How and when should place value concepts and skills be taught? Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 21, 281–286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Baroody, A. J., Eiland, M., Su, Y., & Thompson, B. (2007, April). Fostering at-risk preschoolers’ number sense. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association, Chicago, IL.Google Scholar
  8. Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1988). Young children’s conception of distance. Developmental Psychology, 24(4), 532–541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Beishuizen, M. (1993). Mental strategies and materials or models for addition and subtraction up to 100 in Dutch second grades. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 24, 294–323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Brown, M. C., McNeil, N. M., & Glenberg, A. M. (2009). Using concreteness in education: Real problems, potential solutions. Child Development Perspectives, 3(3), 160–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Brownell, W. A., & Moser, H. E. (1949). Meaningful vs. mechanical learning: A study in grade III subtraction. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Butler, F. M., Miller, S. P., Crehan, K., Babbitt, B., & Pierce, T. (2003). Fraction instruction for students with mathematics disabilities: Comparing two teaching sequences. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 18(2), 99–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Carbonneau, K. J., & Marley, S. C. (2015). Instructional guidance and realism of manipulatives influence preschool children’s mathematics learning. The Journal of Experimental Education, 1–19. doi: 10.1080/00220973.2014.989306 Google Scholar
  14. Carnine, D. W., Jitendra, A. K., & Silbert, J. (1997). A descriptive analysis of mathematics curricular materials from a pedagogical perspective: A case study of fractions. Remedial and Special Education, 18(2), 66–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Carpenter, T. P., & Moser, J. M. (1982). The development of addition and subtraction problem solving. In T. P. Carpenter, J. M. Moser, & T. A. Romberg (Eds.), Rational numbers: An integration of research. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  16. Carpenter, T. P., Ansell, E., Franke, M. L., Fennema, E. H., & Weisbeck, L. (1993). Models of problem solving: A study of kindergarten children’s problem-solving processes. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 24, 428–441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Carr, M., & Alexeev, N. (2011). Developmental trajectories of mathematic strategies: Influence of fluency, accuracy and gender. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 617–631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Char, C. A. (1989, March). Computer graphic feltboards: New software approaches for young children’s mathematical exploration, San Francisco.Google Scholar
  19. Clements, D. H. (1989). Computers in elementary mathematics education. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  20. Clements, D. H. (1999). ‘Concrete’ manipulatives, concrete ideas. Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood, 1(1), 45–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Clements, D. H., & Battista, M. T. (1989). Learning of geometric concepts in a Logo environment. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 20, 450–467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Clements, D. H., & Battista, M. T. (1992). Geometry and spatial reasoning. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 420–464). New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  23. Clements, D. H., & McMillen, S. (1996). Rethinking “concrete” manipulatives. Teaching Children Mathematics, 2(5), 270–279.Google Scholar
  24. Clements, D. H., & Meredith, J. S. (1993). Research on logo: Effects and efficacy. Journal of Computing in Childhood Education, 4, 263–290.Google Scholar
  25. Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2007a). Early childhood mathematics learning. In F. K. Lester Jr. (Ed.), Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (Vol. 1, pp. 461–555). New York, NY: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
  26. Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2007b). Effects of a preschool mathematics curriculum: Summative research on the Building Blocks project. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 38, 136–163.Google Scholar
  27. Clements, D. H., Battista, M. T., & Sarama, J. (2001). Logo and geometry. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education Monograph Series, 10. doi: 10.2307/749924 Google Scholar
  28. Cobb, P. (1995). Cultural tools and mathematical learning: A case study. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 26, 362–385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Cobb, P., Perlwitz, M., & Underwood, D. (1996). Constructivism and activity theory: A consideration of their similarities and differences as they relate to mathematics education. In H. Mansfield, N. A. Pateman, & N. Bednarz (Eds.), Mathematics for tomorrow’s young children (pp. 10–58). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Correa, J., Nunes, T., & Bryant, P. E. (1998). Young children’s understanding of division: The relationship between division terms in a noncomputational task. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 321–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Crollen, V., & Noël, M.-P. (2015). The role of fingers in the development of counting and arithmetic skills. Acta Psychologica, 156, 37–44. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.01.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. De Lange, J. (1987). Mathematics, insight, and meaning. The Netherlands: Utrecht.Google Scholar
  33. DeLoache, J. S., Miller, K. F., Rosengren, K., & Bryant, N. (1997). The credible shrinking room: Very young children’s performance with symbolic and nonsymbolic relations. Psychological Science, 8, 308–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Dewey, J. (1933). How we think: A restatement of the relation of reflective thinking to the educative process. Boston, MA: D. C. Heath and Company.Google Scholar
  35. Diénès, Z. P. (1971). An example of the passage from the concrete to the manipulation of formal systems. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 3(3/4), 337–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Driscoll, M. J. (1983). Research within reach: Elementary school mathematics and reading. St. Louis: CEMREL.Google Scholar
  37. Ernest, P. (1985). The number line as a teaching aid. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 16(4), 411–424. doi: 10.2307/3482448 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Fennema, E. H. (1972). The relative effectiveness of a symbolic and a concrete model in learning a selected mathematics principle. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 3, 233–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Fuson, K. C. (1992a). Research on learning and teaching addition and subtraction of whole numbers. In G. Leinhardt, R. Putman, & R. A. Hattrup (Eds.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 53–187). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  40. Fuson, K. C. (1992b). Research on whole number addition and subtraction. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 243–275). New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  41. Fuson, K. C., & Briars, D. J. (1990). Using a base-ten blocks learning/teaching approach for first- and second-grade place-value and multidigit addition and subtraction. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 21, 180–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Fyfe, E. R., McNeil, N. M., Son, J. Y., & Goldstone, R. L. (2014). Concreteness fading in mathematics and science instruction: A systematic review. Educational Psychology Review, 26(1), 9–25. doi: 10.1007/s10648-014-9249-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Gagatsis, A. (2003). Young children’s understanding of geometric shapes: The role of geometric models. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 11, 43–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Gallou-Dumiel, E. (1989). Reflections, point symmetry and Logo. In C. A. Maher, G. A. Goldin & R. B. Davis (Eds.), Proceedings of the eleventh annual meeting, North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (pp. 149–157). New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University.Google Scholar
  45. Gelman, R. (1994). Constructivism and supporting environments. In D. Tirosh (Ed.), Implicit and explicit knowledge: An educational approach (Vol. 6, pp. 55–82). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  46. Grant, S. G., Peterson, P. L., & Shojgreen-Downer, A. (1996). Learning to teach mathematics in the context of system reform. American Educational Research Journal, 33(2), 509–541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Gravemeijer, K. P. E. (1991). An instruction-theoretical reflection on the use of manipulatives. In L. Streefland (Ed.), Realistic mathematics education in primary school (pp. 57–76). Utrecht, The Netherlands: Freudenthal Institute, Utrecht University.Google Scholar
  48. Gray, E. M., & Pitta, D. (1999). Images and their frames of reference: A perspective on cognitive development in elementary arithmetic. In O. Zaslavsky (Ed.), Proceedings of the 23rd Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 3, pp. 49–56). Haifa, Isreal: Technion.Google Scholar
  49. Greabell, L. C. (1978). The effect of stimuli input on the acquisition of introductory geometric concepts by elementary school children. School Science and Mathematics, 78(4), 320–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Greeno, J. G., & Riley, M. S. (1987). Processes and development of understanding. In R. E. Weinert & R. H. Kluwe (Eds.), Metacognition, motivation, and understanding (pp. 289–313). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  51. Grupe, L. A., & Bray, N. W. (1999, April). What role do manipulatives play in kindergartners’ accuracy and strategy use when solving simple addition problems? Albuquerque, NM.Google Scholar
  52. Guarino, C., Dieterle, S. G., Bargagliotti, A. E., & Mason, W. M. (2013). What can we learn about effective early mathematics teaching? A framework for estimating causal effects using longitudinal survey data. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 6, 164–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Hiebert, J. C., & Wearne, D. (1992). Links between teaching and learning place value with understanding in first grade. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 23, 98–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Hiebert, J. C., & Wearne, D. (1993). Instructional tasks, classroom discourse, and student’ learning in second-grade classrooms. American Educational Research Journal, 30, 393–425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Hiebert, J. C., & Wearne, D. (1996). Instruction, understanding, and skill in multidigit addition and subtraction. Cognition and Instruction, 14, 251–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Holt, J. (1982). How children fail. New York, NY: Dell.Google Scholar
  57. Hughes, M. (1981). Can preschool children add and subtract? Educational Psychology, 1, 207–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Johnson, V. M. (2000, April). An investigation of the effects of instructional strategies on conceptual understanding of young children in mathematics. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA.Google Scholar
  59. Johnson-Gentile, K., Clements, D. H., & Battista, M. T. (1994). The effects of computer and noncomputer environments on students’ conceptualizations of geometric motions. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 11, 121–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Jordan, N. C., Huttenlocher, J., & Levine, S. C. (1994). Assessing early arithmetic abilities: Effects of verbal and nonverbal response types on the calculation performance of middle- and low-income children. Learning and Individual Differences, 6, 413–432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Kamii, C. (1986). Place value: An explanation of its difficulty and educational implications for the primary grades. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 1, 75–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  63. Lamon, W. E., & Huber, L. E. (1971). The learning of the vector space structure by sixth grade students. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 4, 166–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Lane, C. (2010). Case study: The effectiveness of virtual manipulatives in the teaching of primary mathematics (Master thesis, University of Limerick, Limerick, UK). Retrieved from http://digitalcommons.fiu.edu/etd/229
  65. Lehtinen, E., & Hannula, M. M. (2006). Attentional processes, abstraction and transfer in early mathematical development. In L. Verschaffel, F. Dochy, M. Boekaerts, & S. Vosniadou (Eds.), Instructional psychology: Past, present and future trends. Fifteen essays in honour of Erik De Corte (Vol. 49, pp. 39–55). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  66. Lesh, R. A. (1990). Computer-based assessment of higher order understandings and processes in elementary mathematics. In G. Kulm (Ed.), Assessing higher order thinking in mathematics (pp. 81–110). Washington, DC: American Association for the Advancement of Science.Google Scholar
  67. Lesh, R. A., & Johnson, H. (1976). Models and applications as advanced organizers. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 7, 75–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Levine, S. C., Jordan, N. C., & Huttenlocher, J. (1992). Development of calculation abilities in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 53, 72–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. MacDonald, A., Davies, N., Dockett, S., & Perry, B. (2012). Early childhood mathematics education. In B. Perry, T. Lowrie, T. Logan, A. MacDonald, & J. Greenlees (Eds.), Research in mathematics education in Australasia: 2008–2011 (pp. 169–192). Rotterdam, The Netherlands: Sense Publishers.Google Scholar
  70. Mandler, J. M. (2004). The foundations of mind: Origins of conceptual thought. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  71. Martin, T. (2009). A theory of physically distributed learning: How external environments and internal states interact in mathematics learning. Child Development Perspectives, 3(3), 140–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Martin, T., Lukong, A., & Reaves, R. (2007). The role of manipulatives in arithmetic and geometry tasks. Journal of Education and Human Development, 1(1).Google Scholar
  73. Metz, K. E. (1995). Reassessment of developmental constraints on children’s science instruction. Review of Educational Research, 65, 93–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Miura, I. T., & Okamoto, Y. (2003). Language supports for mathematics understanding and performance. In A. J. Baroody & A. Dowker (Eds.), The development of arithmetic concepts and skills: Constructing adaptive expertise (pp. 229–242). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  75. Moyer, P. S. (2000). Are we having fun yet? Using manipulatives to teach “real math”. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 47, 175–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Moyer, P. S., Niezgoda, D., & Stanley, J. (2005). Young children’s use of virtual manipulatives and other forms of mathematical representations. In W. Masalski & P. C. Elliott (Eds.), Technology-supported mathematics learning environments: 67th Yearbook (pp. 17–34). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.Google Scholar
  77. Moyer-Packenham, P. S., & Westenskow, A. (2013). Effects of virtual manipulatives on student achievement and mathematics learning. International Journal of Virtual and Personal Learning Environments, 4(3), 35–50. doi: 10.4018/jvple.2013070103 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Moyer-Packenham, P. S., Baker, J., Westenskow, A., Anderson-Pence, K. L., Shumway, J. F., Rodzon, K., et al. (2013). A study comparing virtual manipulatives with other instructional treatments in third- and fourth-grade classrooms. Journal of Education and Human Development, 193(2), 25–39.Google Scholar
  79. Moyer-Packenham, P. S., Shumway, J. F., Bullock, E., Tucker, S. I., Anderson-Pence, K. L., Westenskow, A., et al. (2015). Young children’s learning performance and efficiency when using virtual manipulative mathematics iPad apps. Journal of Computers in Mathematics and Science Teaching, 34(1), 41–69.Google Scholar
  80. Munn, P. (1998). Symbolic function in pre-schoolers. In C. Donlan (Ed.), The development of mathematical skills (pp. 47–71). East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  81. Murata, A. (2008). Mathematics teaching and learning as a mediating process: The case of tape diagrams. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 10, 374–406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. National Mathematics Advisory Panel. (2008). Foundations for success: The final report of the National Mathematics Advisory Panel. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, Office of Planning, Evaluation and Policy Development.Google Scholar
  83. Ng, S. N. S., & Rao, N. (2010). Chinese number words, culture, and mathematics learning. Review of Educational Research, 80(2), 180–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Nishida, T. K., & Lillard, A. S. (2007a, April). From flashcard to worksheet: Children’s inability to transfer across different formats. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
  85. Nishida, T. K., & Lillard, A. S. (2007b, April). Fun toy or learning tool?: Young children’s use of concrete manipulatives to learn about simple math concepts. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
  86. Núãez, R., Cooperrider, K., & Wassmann, J. (2012). Number concepts without number lines in an indigenous group of papua New Guinea. PLoS ONE, 7(4), 1–8. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035662 Google Scholar
  87. Olson, J. K. (1988, August). Microcomputers make manipulatives meaningful. Paper presented at the International Congress of Mathematics Education, Budapest, Hungary.Google Scholar
  88. Outhred, L. N., & Sardelich, S. (1997). Problem solving in kindergarten: The development of representations. In F. Biddulph & K. Carr (Eds.), People in Mathematics Education. Proceedings of the 20th Annual Conference of the Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia (Vol. 2, pp. 376–383). Rotorua, New Zealand: Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia.Google Scholar
  89. Palardy, G., & Rumberger, R. (2008). Teacher effectiveness in first grade: The importance of background qualifications, attitudes, and instructional practices for student learning. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 30, 111–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Rao, N., Ng, S. N. S., & Pearson, E. (2009). Preschool pedagogy: A fusion of traditional Chinese beliefs and contemporary notions of appropriate practice. In C. K. K. Chan & N. Rao (Eds.), Revisiting the Chinese learner: Changing contexts, changing education (pp. 211–231). Hong Kong: University of Hong Kong, Comparative Education Research Center/Springer Academic.Google Scholar
  91. Raphael, D., & Wahlstrom, M. (1989). The influence of instructional aids on mathematics achievement. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 20, 173–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Reimer, K., & Moyer, P. S. (2004, April). A classroom study of third-graders’ use of virtual manipulatives to learn about fractions. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association, San Diego, CA.Google Scholar
  93. Resnick, L. B., & Omanson, S. (1987). Learning to understand arithmetic. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology (pp. 41–95). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  94. Sarama, J., & Clements, D. H. (2006). Mathematics, young students, and computers: Software, teaching strategies and professional development. The Mathematics Educator, 9(2), 112–134.Google Scholar
  95. Sarama, J., & Clements, D. H. (2009a). “Concrete” computer manipulatives in mathematics education. Child Development Perspectives, 3(3), 145–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Sarama, J., & Clements, D. H. (2009b). Early childhood mathematics education research: Learning trajectories for young children. New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
  97. Sarama, J., Clements, D. H., & Vukelic, E. B. (1996). The role of a computer manipulative in fostering specific psychological/mathematical processes. In E. Jakubowski, D. Watkins & H. Biske (Eds.), Proceedings of the 18th Annual Meeting of the North America Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 2, pp. 567–572). Columbus, OH: ERIC Clearinghouse for Science, Mathematics, and Environmental Education.Google Scholar
  98. Schliemann, A. D., Carraher, D. W., & Brizuela, B. M. (2007). Bringing out the algebraic character of arithmetic. Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  99. Sedighian, K., & Klawe, M. M. (1996). Super Tangrams: A child-centered approach to designing a computer supported mathematics learning environment. In Proceedings of the ICLS (pp. 490–495).Google Scholar
  100. Sedighian, K., & Sedighian, A. (1996). Can educational computer games help educators learn about the psychology of learning mathematics in children? In E. Jakubowski, D. Watkins & H. Biske (Eds.), Proceedings of the 18th Annual Meeting of the North America Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 2, pp. 573–578). Columbus, OH: ERIC Clearinghouse for Science, Mathematics, and Environmental Education.Google Scholar
  101. Seo, K.-H., & Ginsburg, H. P. (2004). What is developmentally appropriate in early childhood mathematics education? In D. H. Clements, J. Sarama, & A.-M. DiBiase (Eds.), Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood mathematics education (pp. 91–104). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  102. Sherman, J., & Bisanz, J. (2009). Equivalence in symbolic and non-symbolic contexts: Benefits of solving problems with manipulatives. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(1), 88–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Skoumpourdi, C. (2010). Kindergarten mathematics with ‘Pepe the Rabbit’: how kindergartners use auxiliary means to solve problems. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 18(3), 149–157. doi: 10.1080/1350293x.2010.500070 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Sowell, E. J. (1989). Effects of manipulative materials in mathematics instruction. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 20, 498–505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Spelke, E. S. (2003). What makes us smart? Core knowledge and natural language. In D. Genter & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), Language in mind (pp. 277–311). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  106. Spitler, M. E., Sarama, J., & Clements, D. H. (2003, April). A preschooler’s understanding of “triangle:” A case study. Paper presented at the 81th Annual Meeting of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, San Antonio, TX.Google Scholar
  107. Steffe, L. P., & Cobb, P. (1988). Construction of arithmetical meanings and strategies. New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Suydam, M. N. (1986). Manipulative materials and achievement. Arithmetic Teacher, 33(6), 10, 32.Google Scholar
  109. Thompson, A. C. (2012). The effect of enhanced visualization instruction on first grade students’ scores on the North Carolina standard course assessment (Dissertation, Liberty University, Lynchburg, VA).Google Scholar
  110. Thompson, P. W. (1992). Notations, conventions, and constraints: Contributions to effective use of concrete materials in elementary mathematics. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 23, 123–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Thompson, P. W., & Thompson, A. G. (1990). Salient aspects of experience with concrete manipulatives. In F. Hitt (Ed.), Proceedings of the 14th Annual Meeting of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics (Vol. 3, pp. 337–343). Mexico City: International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education.Google Scholar
  112. Uttal, D. H., Marzolf, D. P., Pierroutsakos, S. L., Smith, C. M., Troseth, G. L., & Scudder, K. V. (1997a). Seeing through symbols: The development of children’s understanding of symbolic relations. In O. N. Saracho & B. Spodek (Eds.), Multiple perspectives on play in early childhood education (pp. 59–79). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
  113. Uttal, D. H., Scudder, K. V., & DeLoache, J. S. (1997b). Manipulatives as symbols: A new perspective on the use of concrete objects to teach mathematics. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 18, 37–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  114. van Hiele, P. M. (1986). Structure and insight: A theory of mathematics education. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  115. van Oers, B. (1994). Semiotic activity of young children in play: The construction and use of schematic representations. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 2, 19–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  116. Varela, F. J. (1999). Ethical know-how: Action, wisdom, and cognition. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  117. Vitale, J. M., Black, J. B., & Swart, M. I. (2014). Applying grounded coordination challenges to concrete learning materials: A study of number line estimation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(2), 403–418. doi: 10.1037/a0034098 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  118. Vygotsky, L. S. (1934/1986). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  119. Wilensky, U. (1991). Abstract meditations on the concrete and concrete implications for mathematics education. In I. Harel & S. Papert (Eds.), Constructionism (pp. 193–199). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  120. Yerushalmy, M. (2005). Functions of interactive visual representations in interactive mathematical textbooks. International Journal of Computers for Mathematical Learning, 10, 217–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of DenverDenverUSA

Personalised recommendations