Advertisement

A biological basis for musical tonality

  • Daniel Bowling
  • Dale Purves

Abstract

Like other sensory qualities, the human ability to perceive tonal sound stimuli has presumably evolved because of its utility. Although a variety of tonal sounds are present in the human auditory environment, the vocalizations of other humans are the most biologically relevant and the most frequently experienced. It is thus reasonable to assume that our appreciation of tonal sounds has arisen primarily for the benefits that accrue from this conspecific information. It follows that the structure and function of the tonal sounds produced by the human vocal apparatus may provide the key to understanding how and why we perceive tonality in music the way that we do. Here we consider recent evidence that bears on this idea.

Keywords

Fundamental Frequency Chromatic Scale Minor Scale Pitch Height Musical Tonality 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Aldwell E, Schachter C (2003) Harmony & voice leading, 3rd edn. Wadsworth Group/Thomson Learning, Belmont, CAGoogle Scholar
  2. Banse R, Scherer KR (1996) Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. J Pers Soc Psychol 70: 614–636PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Barlow H, Morgenstern S (1974) A dictionary of musical themes. Crown, New York, NYGoogle Scholar
  4. Bernstein L (1976) The unanswered question: six talks at Harvard. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  5. Bowling DL, Gill K, Choi JD, Prinz J, Purves D (2010) Major and minor music compared to excited and subdued speech. J Acoust Soc Am 127: in pressGoogle Scholar
  6. Burkholder JP, Grout D, Palisca C (2006) A history of Western music, 7th edn. Norton, New York, NYGoogle Scholar
  7. Burns EM (1999) Intervals, scales and tuning. In: Deutsch D (ed) The psychology of music, 2nd edn. Academic Press, New York, NYGoogle Scholar
  8. Cohen D (1971) Palestrina counterpoint: A musical expression of unexcited speech. J Music Theory 15: 85–111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cooke D (1959) The language of music. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UKGoogle Scholar
  10. Crowder RG (1984) Perception of the major/minor distinction: hedonic, musical, and affective discriminations. B Psychonomic Soc 23: 314–316Google Scholar
  11. Crystal D (1997) The Cambridge encyclopedia of language, 2nd edn. Cambridge University Press, New York, NYGoogle Scholar
  12. Delattre P, Liberman AM, Cooper FS, Gerstman LJ (1952) An experimental study of the acoustic determinants of vowel color: observation of one-and two-formant vowels synthesized from spectrographic patterns. Word 8: 195–210Google Scholar
  13. Eerola T, Tovianien P (2004) Suomen Kasan eSävelmät (Finnish folk song database). Available from http://www. jyu.fi/musica/sks/ in November, 2008Google Scholar
  14. Gill K, Purves D (2009) A biological rationale for musical scales. PLoS ONE 4, e8144.doi:101371/ journal.pone.0008144PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gregory AH, Varney N (1996) Cross-cultural comparisons in the affective response to music. Psychol Music 24: 47–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Hammerschmidt K, Jürgens U (2007) Acoustical correlates of affective prosody. J Voice 21: 531–540PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Harrington J, Palethorpe S, Watson CI (2007) Agerelated changes in fundamental frequency and formants: a longitudinal study of four speakers. In: Proceedings of Interspeech 2007, AntwerpGoogle Scholar
  18. Heinlein CP (1928) The affective characters of the major and minor modes in music. J Comp Psychol 8: 101–142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Helmholtz H (1885) Lehre von den Tonempfindungen, 4th Edn., translated by Ellis AJ (1954) as: On the sensations of tone, 2nd edn. Dover, New York, NYGoogle Scholar
  20. Hevner K (1935) The affective character of the major and minor modes in music. Am J Psychol 47: 103–118CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hillenbrand J, Getty LA, Clark MJ, Wheeler K (1995) Acoustic characteristics of American English vowels. J Acoust Soc Am 97: 3099–3111PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hollien H (1960) Some laryngeal correlates of vocal pitch. J Speech Hear Res 3: 52–58PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Johnstone T, Scherer KR (2000) Vocal communication of emotion. In: Lewis M, and Haviland-Jones JM (eds) Handbook of Emotions, 2nd edn. Guilford, New York, NYGoogle Scholar
  24. Juslin PN, Laukka P (2003) Communication of emotions in vocal expression and music performance: Different channels, same code? Psychol Bull 129: 770–814PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Krumhansl CL (1979) The psychological representation of musical pitch in tonal context. Cognitive Psychol 11: 346–374CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Krumhansl CL (1990) Cognitive foundations for musical pitch. Oxford University Press, New York, NYGoogle Scholar
  27. Patel AD (2008) Music, language, and the brain. Oxford University Press, New York, NYGoogle Scholar
  28. Peretz I, Gagnon L, Bouchard B (1998) Music and emotion: perceptual determinants, immediacy, and isolation after brain damage. Cognition 68: 111–141PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Petersen GE, Barney HL (1962) Control methods used in a study of the vowels. J Acoust Soc Am 24: 175–184CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Pickett JM (1957) Perception of vowels heard in noises of various spectra. J Acoust Soc Am 29: 613–620CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Pierce JR (1962) The science of musical sound, revised edn. Freeman, New York, NYGoogle Scholar
  32. Protopapas A, Lieberman P (1996) Fundamental frequency of phonation and perceived emotional stress. J Acoust Soc Am 101: 2267–2277CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Randel DM (ed) (1986) The new Harvard dictionary of music, revised 2nd edn. Belknap Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  34. Rosner BS, Pickering JB (1994) Vowel perception and production. Oxford University Press, New York, NYCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Ross D, Choi J, Purves D (2007) Musical intervals in speech. Proc Natl Acad Sci 104(23): 9852–9857PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Rossing TD (1990) The science of sound, 2nd edn. Addison-Wesley, New York, NYGoogle Scholar
  37. Scherer KR (2003) Vocal communication of emotion: A review of research paradigms. Speech Commun 40: 227–256CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Scherer KR, Banse R, Wallbott HG (2001) Emotional inferences from vocal expression correlate across languages and cultures. J Cross Cult Psychol 32: 76–92CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Schwartz DA, Purves D (2004) Pitch is determined by naturally occurring periodic sounds. Hearing Res 194: 31–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Schwartz DA, Howe CQ, Purves D (2003) The statistical structure of human speech sounds predicts musical universals. J Neurosci 23: 7160–7168PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Spencer H (1857) The origin and function of music. Fraser’s Magazine 56: 396–408Google Scholar
  42. Terhardt E (1974) Pitch, consonance, and harmony. J Acoust Soc Am 55: 1061–1069PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Terhardt E (1984) The concept of musical consonance: A link between music and psychoacoustics. Music Percept 1: 276–295CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Zarlino G (1558) Bk. 3 of Le institutioni harmoniche, translated by Marco G, Palisca C (1968) as The Art of Counterpoint. Yale University Press, New Haven, CTGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag/Wien 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • Daniel Bowling
    • 1
  • Dale Purves
    • 1
  1. 1.NeurobiologyDuke-NUS Graduate Medical SchoolSingaporeSingapore

Personalised recommendations