Advertisement

A Computational Model for Distance Perception Based on Visual Attention Redeployment in Locomotor Infants

  • Liz A. Jaramillo–HenaoEmail author
  • Adrián A. Vélez–Aristizábal
  • Jaime E. Arango–Castro
Conference paper
Part of the Communications in Computer and Information Science book series (CCIS, volume 1096)

Abstract

Self-locomotion experience of infants has been argued to improve perception of distance, as visual attention is drawn to previously undetected or ignored depth specifying information. We present a computational model to evaluate how does self-locomotion experience influences the estimation of distance in infants. The model assigns an estimated distance label to salient objects in the scene, through a Binocular Neural Network (BNN) that computes binocular disparities. Emphasizing on key aspects of locomotion experience, two BNN are trained, one for non-locomotor infants and one for locomotor infants. The validation and test stages of the process show a significant improvement on the distance estimation task for the BNN trained with locomotor experience. This result is added to previous evidence which supports that locomotion in infants is an important step in cognitive development.

Keywords

Cognitive Developmental Robotics Visual attention Locomotor experience Binocular Neural Network 

Notes

Acknowledgment

We would like to express our gratitude to the Soft and Hard Computing (SHAC) research group from the Universidad Nacional de Colombia, Manizales, for their valuable suggestions for this work. We also thank the Electrical and Electronics Engineering Department, and the Faculty of Engineering and Architecture for its support.

References

  1. 1.
    Asada, M., MacDorman, K.F., Ishiguro, H., Kuniyoshi, Y.: Cognitive developmental robotics as a new paradigm for the design of humanoid robots. Rob. Auton. Syst. 37, 185–193 (2001)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. 2.
    Asada, M., et al.: Cognitive developmental robotics: a survey. IEEE Trans. Auton. Ment. Dev. 1, 12–34 (2009)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. 3.
    Fujikado, T., Approaches, S.: Cognitive Neuroscience Robotics A. Springer, Japan (2016).  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-54595-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. 4.
    Thompson, B., et al.: Global motion perception is associated with motor function in 2-year-old children. Neurosci. Lett. 658, 177–181 (2017)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    Cicchino, J.B., Rakison, D.H.: Producing and processing self-propelled motion in infancy. Dev. Psychol. 44, 1232–1241 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Brand, R.J., Escobar, K., Baranés, A., Albu, A.: Crawling predicts infants’ understanding of agents’ navigation of obstacles. Infancy 20, 405–415 (2015)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. 7.
    Kubicek, C., Jovanovic, B., Schwarzer, G.: The relation between crawling and 9-month-old infants’ visual prediction abilities in spatial object processing. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 158, 64–76 (2017)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    Forma, V., Anderson, D.I., Goffinet, F., Barbu-Roth, M.: Effect of optic flows on newborn crawling. Dev. Psychobiol. 60, 497–510 (2018)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    Gerhard, T.M., Schwarzer, G.: Impact of rotation angle on crawling and non-crawling 9-month-old infants’ mental rotation ability. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 170, 45–56 (2018)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    Campos, J.J., Anderson, D.I., Barbu-Roth, M.A., Hubbard, E.M., Hertenstein, M.J., Witherington, D.: Travel broadens the mind. Infancy 1, 149–219 (2000)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Daw, N.W.: Visual Development. Springer, Boston (2014).  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-9059-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.
    Gustafson, G.E.: Effects of the ability to locomote on infants’ social and exploratory behaviors: an experimental study. Dev. Psychol. 20, 397–405 (1984)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Mann, T.A., Park, Y., Jeong, S., Lee, M., Choe, Y.: Autonomous and interactive improvement of binocular visual depth estimation through sensorimotor interaction. IEEE Trans. Auton. Ment. Dev. 5, 74–84 (2013)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. 14.
    Goncalves, N.R., Welchman, A.E.: “What Not” detectors help the brain see in depth. Curr. Biol. 27, 1403–1412.e8 (2017)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. 15.
    Harnad, S.: The symbol grounding problem. Phys. D Nonlinear Phenom. 42, 335–346 (1990)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. 16.
    Nagai, Y.: The role of motion information in learning human-robot joint attention. In: Proceedings-IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation 2005, pp. 2069–2074 (2005)Google Scholar
  17. 17.
    Nagai, Y., Hosoda, K., Morita, A., Asada, M.: Emergence of joint attention based on visual attention and self learning. In: Learning, pp. 2–7 (2003)Google Scholar
  18. 18.
    Nagai, Y., Kawai, Y., Asada, M.: Emergence of mirror neuron system: immature vision leads to self-other correspondence. In: 2011 IEEE International Conference Development and Learning, ICDL 2011 (2011)Google Scholar
  19. 19.
    Baraglia, J., Nagai, Y., Kawai, Y., Asada, M.: The role of temporal variance in motions for the emergence of mirror neurons systems. In: 2012 IEEE International Conference on Development and Learning and Epigenetic Robotics, ICDL 2012, pp. 1–2 (2012)Google Scholar
  20. 20.
    Kawai, Y., Nagai, Y., Asada, M.: Perceptual development triggered by its self-organization in cognitive learning. In: 2012 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems, pp. 5159–5164. IEEE (2012)Google Scholar
  21. 21.
    Nagai, Y., Asada, M.: Predictive learning of sensorimotor information as a key for cognitive development. In: Proceedings of the IROS 2015 Workshop on Sensorimotor Contingencies for Robotics (2015)Google Scholar
  22. 22.
    Dodgson, N.A.: Variation and extrema of human interpupillary distance. In: Stereoscopic Displays and Virtual Reality Systems XI, pp. 36–46 (2004)Google Scholar
  23. 23.
    Dominguez, M., Jacobs, R.A.: Developmental constraints aid the acquisition of binocular disparity sensitivities. Neural Comput. 15, 161–182 (2003)CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Liz A. Jaramillo–Henao
    • 1
    Email author
  • Adrián A. Vélez–Aristizábal
    • 1
  • Jaime E. Arango–Castro
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Electrical and Electronics EngineeringNational University of ColombiaManizalesColombia

Personalised recommendations