Advertisement

The Semiotic Resources Children Use in Their Explanations of Hypothetical Situations

  • Elena SeverinaEmail author
  • Tamsin Meaney
Chapter
  • 31 Downloads

Abstract

The aim of the study is to describe the semiotic resources that children used in mathematical explanations of hypothetical situations. The empirical material consists of a short video from a Norwegian kindergarten, where four 5-year-old children and a kindergarten student teacher discuss real and imaginary page layouts of a photo book. When explaining their reasoning about the amount of photographs in the layouts, the children used oral language, gestures and physical objects like number charts. The use of these resources in different kinds of explanations is discussed. Our results suggest that by using a range of semiotic resources, children are able to provide explanations of hypothetical situations using mathematical ideas at a younger age than previously suggested.

Keywords

Semiotic resources Explanations Hypothetical situations Imaginary objects Gestures 

References

  1. Alibali, M. W., & Nathan, M. J. (2012). Embodiment in mathematics teaching and learning: Evidence from learners’ and teachers’ gestures. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 21(2), 247–286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Donaldson, M. L. (1986). Children’s explanations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Edwards, L. D. (2009). Gestures and conceptual integration in mathematical talk. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70(2), 127–141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Gelman, R., & Gallistel, C. (1978). The child’s understanding of number. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Goldin-Meadow, S. (1998). The development of gesture and speech as an integrated system. New Directions for Child Development, 79, 29–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Johansson, M., Lange, T., Meaney, T., Riesbeck, E., & Wernberg, A. (2014). Young children’s multimodal mathematical explanations. ZDM—International Journal on Mathematics Education, 46(6), 895–909.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  8. McNeill, D. (2005). Gesture and thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Meaney, T. (2011). Only two more sleeps until the school holidays: One child’s home experiences of measurement. For the Learning of Mathematics, 31(1), 31–36.Google Scholar
  10. Radford, L., & Sabena, C. (2015). The question of method in a vygotskian semiotic approach. In A. Bikner-Ahsbahs, C. Knipping, & N. Presmeg (Eds.), Approaches to qualitative research in mathematics education (pp. 157–182). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  11. Roth, W.-M. (2001). Gestures: their role in teaching and learning. Review of Educational Research, 71(3), 365–392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Sabena, C. (2008). On the semiotics of gestures. In L. Radford, G. Schubring, & F. Seeger (Eds.), Semiotics in mathematics education: Epistemology, history, classroom, and culture (pp. 19–38). Rotterdam: Sense Publishers.Google Scholar
  13. Saar, T. (2013). Articulating the immanent: Children unfolding numbers. Global Studies of Childhood, 3(3), 310–317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Yackel, E. (2001). Explanation, justification and argumentation in mathematics classrooms. In A. Cockburn & E. Nardi (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th Annual Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 1, pp. 9–25). Norwich: PME.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2020

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Western Norway University of Applied SciencesBergenNorway

Personalised recommendations