Advertisement

Third Principle: Dynamics of Involvement and Self-Transformation

  • Danilo Silva Guimarães
Chapter
Part of the Latin American Voices book series (LAVIPH)

Abstract

The term ‘psychology’ was created by humanist renaissance thinkers who, recovering the Greek myth of Psyche, prepared a rational elaboration of some human dimensions referred in the myth and amplified it. The indigenous psychology here proposed is founded on the recognition that a mythical basis underlies all knowledge constructions, and that other myths besides the Greek-Roman and Judeo-Christian ones should participate in this process. The history of interethnic relations is marked by ignorance concerning indigenous peoples’ concepts of person. The notion of person, as it is discussed in each cultural field shows clearly the existence of distinct debate arenas, and a gap between the different dialogical spaces as a nebulous and unknown region. Then, personal self is the person’s symbolic shelter for diversity, a construct that refers to an active field of the person, occupied and cultivated by the culture, with borders that are semi-permeable to dialogue. To a certain point, the person resists the culture’s interventions, gaining some autonomy to direct their own paths in the process of transforming themselves and the world. Besides, the affective body’s activities in the environment are coordinated with others; there is a certain regularity in the different spheres of life. This chapter focuses on how culture cultivates psychological systems through investments that imply the construction of personal self and corporeity.

Keywords

Affective body Resembling the other Cultural rhythms Myths Ethos Nudity 

Notes

Acknowledgement

This work is supported by FAPESP (grant number 18/13145-0).

References

  1. Baccaert, A., Navarro, C., & Um, N. (2011). À Sombra de um Delírio Verde. Recuperado de http://www.thedarksideofgreen-themovie.com/.
  2. Bakhtin, M. M. (1929/2013). Problemas da poética de Dostoiévski (P. Bezerra, trad.), 5a. ed., 3a. imp. Rio de Janeiro: Forense Universitária.Google Scholar
  3. Bakhtin, M. M. (1979/2015). Estética da criação verbal (P. Bezerra, trad.), 6a. ed., 2a. tiragem. São Paulo: Martins Fontes.Google Scholar
  4. Bakhtin, M. (1986). Speech genres & other late essays. Caryl Emerson e Michael Holquist (Orgs.). Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
  5. Baldwin, J. M. (1906a). Mental development in the child and the race (3rd ed.). New York: Macmillan. Retrieved from http://www.brocku.ca/MeadProject/Baldwin/Baldwin_1906/Baldwin_1906_toc.html.Google Scholar
  6. Baldwin, J. M. (1906b). Thought and things: A study of the development and meaning of thought, or genetic logic (Functional logic, or genetic theory of knowledge) (Vol. 1). London: Swan Sonnenschein & Co.Google Scholar
  7. Barrera, J. S. (2016) Tzotzil person model: A guide to understanding indigenous people’s behavior and thought. In Guimarães, D. S. (Org.) Amerindian paths: Guiding dialogues with psychology (pp. 7–24). Charlotte: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
  8. Boesch, E. E. (1984). The development of affective schemata. Human Development, 27(3–4), 173–183.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Boesch, E. E. (1991). Symbolic action theory and cultural psychology. Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Boesch, E. E. (1993). The sound of the violin. Revue Suisse de Psychologie, 52(2), 70–81.Google Scholar
  11. Boesch, E. E. (1997). Reasons for a Symbolic concept of action. Culture & Psychology, 3(3), 423–431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Boesch, E. E. (2007). Culture – individual – culture: The cycle of knowledge. In W. J. Lonner & S. A. Hayes (Eds.), Discovering cultural psychology: A profile and selected readinds of Ernest E. Boesch (pp. 201–212). Charlotte: Information Age Publishing. (Originally published in 1992).Google Scholar
  13. Chauí, M. S. (1980). O que é ideologia? São Paulo: `Brasiliense.Google Scholar
  14. Descartes, R. (1641/2004). Meditações sobre filosofia primeira (F. Castilho, trad.). Campinas: Unicamp. Disponível In http://charlezine.com.br/wp-content/uploads/2012/04/Medita%C3%A7%C3%B5es-sobre-Filosofia-Primeira-Ren%C3%A9-Descartes.pdf.
  15. Figueiredo, L. C. M. (2013). Revisitando as psicologias: da epistemologia à ética das práticas e discursos psicológicos (p. 183). Petropólis, Vozes. (Trabalho original publicado em 1996).Google Scholar
  16. Gonçalves, B. S. (2016) Neither Tupi nor Tapuia. Free determination and social policies in the historic trajectory of the Brazilian indigenous peoples. In: Guimarães, D. S. (org.) Amerindian paths: Guiding dialogues with psychology (pp. 25–44). Charlotte: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
  17. Gow, P. (1997). O parentesco como consciência humana: o caso dos Piro. Mana, 3(2), 39–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Guimarães, D. S. (2013). Self and dialogical multiplication. Interacções, 9, 214–242.Google Scholar
  19. Guimarães, D. S. (2015). Temporality as reciprocity of activities: Articulating the cyclical and the irreversible in personal symbolic transformations. In: Simão, L. M., Guimarães, D. S. & Valsiner, J. (orgs.). Temporality: Culture in the flow of human experience (pp. 331–358). Charlotte: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
  20. Guimarães, D. S. (2016a). Amerindian paths: Guiding dialogues with psychology (p. 366). Charlotte: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
  21. Guimarães, D. S. (2016b). Descending and ascending trajectories of dialogical analysis: Seventh analytic interpretation on the short story “The guerrillero”. Psicologia USP, 27(2), 189–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Guimarães, D. S. (2017). Dialogical mutiplication: Essays on cultural psychology (Tese de Livre-Docência). Instituto de Psicologia, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo.Google Scholar
  23. Guimarães, D. S. & Cravo, A. M. (2015). Understanding others without a word: Articulating the shared circuits model with semiotic-cultural constructivist psychology. In: Beckstead, Z. (org.). Cultural psychology of recursive processes (pp. 143–160). Charlotte: IAP – Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
  24. Guimarães, D. S., & Nash, R. (in preparation). O que nos comunica o ritmo? Reflexões a partir do construtivismo semiótico-cultural em psicologia. (Manuscript in preparation). São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo.Google Scholar
  25. Guimarães, D. S. & Simão, L. M. (2007). Intersubjectivity and otherness: A stage for self strivings. In: Simão, L. M. & Valsiner, J. (Orgs.). Otherness in question: Labyrinths of the self (pp. 317–347). Greenwich: Information Age Publishing Inc.Google Scholar
  26. Guimarães, D. S. & Simão, L. M. (2017) Mythological constrains to the construction of subjectified bodies. In: Han, M. (Org.) The subjectified and subjectifying mind (pp. 3–21). Charlotte: Information Age Publication.Google Scholar
  27. Guimarães, D. S., Lima Neto, D. M., Soares, L. M., Santos, P. D., & Carvalho, T. S. (2019). Temporalidade e corpo numa proposta de formação do psicólogo para o trabalho com povos indígenas. [Temporality and body in proposal for training psychologist in the work with indigenous peoples]. Psicologia: Ciência e Profissão, 39.  https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-3703003221929.
  28. Hermans, H. J. M. (2001). The dialogical self: Toward a theory of personal and cultural positioning. Culture & Psychology, 7(3), 243–281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Holquist, M. (1990). Dialogism – Bakhtin and his world. London/New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Ideti & Duwe, E. (2010). Tenonderã ‑ Um olhar para o Futuro [Documentário on-line]. São Paulo: Recuperado de http://vimeo.com/20263900.
  31. Ingold, T. (2000). The perception of the environment: Essays on livelihood, dwelling and skill. Oxon/New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  32. Johansen, J. D. (2010). Feelings in literature. Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science, 44(3), 185–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Krstic, K. (1964). Marko Marulic the author of the term “Psychology”. Acta Instituti Psychologici Universitatis Zagrabiensis, 36, 7–13.Google Scholar
  34. Lagrou, E. (2007). A fluidez da forma: Arte, alteridade e agência em uma sociedade amazônica (Kaxinawa, Acre). Rio de Janeiro: Topbooks.Google Scholar
  35. Langer, S. K. (1953). Feeling and form: A theory of art. New York: Scribner’s.Google Scholar
  36. Lévi-Strauss, C., & Eribon, D. (1990). De perto e de longe: Reflexões do mais importante antropólogo do nosso tempo. Rio de Janeiro: Nova Fronteira. (Texto original publicado em 1988).Google Scholar
  37. Lima, T. S. (2005). Um peixe olhou pra mim: o povo Yudjá e a perspectiva. São Paulo: UNESP.Google Scholar
  38. Morais, H. Z. L., & Guimarães, D. S. (2015). Borders of poetic self construction: Dialogues between cultural psychology and performing arts. Psychology & Society, 7, 28–39.Google Scholar
  39. Rommetveit, R. (1979) On negative rationalism in scholarly studies of verbal communication and dynamic residuals in the construction of human intersubjectivity. In: R. Rommetveit; R. Blakar (Orgs.) Studies of language, thought and verbal communication (pp. 147–161). London: Academic.Google Scholar
  40. Rommetveit, R. (1992). Outlines of a dialogically based socio-cognitive approach to human cognition and communication. In A. H. Wold (Ed.), The dialogical alternative: Towards a theory of language and mind (pp. 19–44). Oslo: Scandinavian University Press.Google Scholar
  41. Seeger, A., Da Matta, R., & Viveiros de Castro, E. B. (1979). A construção da pessoa nas sociedades indígenas brasileiras. Boletim do Museu Nacional, 32, 2–19.Google Scholar
  42. Setton, M. d. G. J. (2002). A teoria do habitus em Pierre Bourdieu: Uma leitura contemporânea. Revista Brasileira de Educação, 20, 60–70.  https://doi.org/10.1590/S1413-24782002000200005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Simão, L. M. (2010). Ensaios Dialógicos: Compartilhamento e diferença nas relações eu outro (p. 286). São Paulo: HUCITEC.Google Scholar
  44. Simão, L. M., & Valsiner, J. (2007). Multiple faces of otherness within the infinite labyrinths of the self. In L. M. Simão & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Otherness in question: Labyrinths of the self (pp. 393–406). Charlotte: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
  45. Sobral, A. (2014) Ato/atividade e evento. In Brait, B. (Org.), Bakhtin: Conceitos-chave (pp.11–36). São Paulo: Contexto.Google Scholar
  46. Strathern, M. (1991). One men and many men. In M. Godelier & M. Strathern (Eds.), Big men and great men: Personifications of power in Melanesia. Cambridge: Maison des Sciences de l’Homme and Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  47. Taylor, A.-C. (1984/1996). The soul’s body and its states: An Amazonian perspective on the nature of being human. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute (N.S.), 2(2), 201–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Valsiner, J. (1994). Irreversibility of time and the construction of historical developmental psychology. Mind, culture and activity, 1(1/2), 24–42.Google Scholar
  49. Valsiner, J. (2002). Irreversibility of time and the ontopotentiality of signs. Estudios de Psicología, 23(1), 49–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Valsiner, J. (2007). Human development as migration: Striving toward the unknown. In L. M. Simão & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Otherness in question: Labyrinths of the self (pp. 349–378). Charlotte: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
  51. Valsiner, J. (2008). Baldwin’s quest: A universal logic of development. In J. Clegg (Ed.), The observation of human systems: Lessons from the history of anti-reductionist empirical psychology (pp. 45–82). New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
  52. Viveiros de Castro, E. B. (2006). A inconstância da alma selvagem e outros ensaios de antropologia (2nd ed.). São Paulo: Cosac Naify. (Original publicado em 2002).Google Scholar
  53. Vygotski, L. S. (1934a). La imaginación y su desarrollo en la edad infantil. In L. S. Vygotski (Ed.), Obras escogidas (Tomo II) (pp. 423–438). Madrid: A. Machado Libros, S. A.Google Scholar
  54. Vygotski, L. S. (1934b). Pensamiento y lenguaje. In Obras escogidas (Tomo II) (pp. 11–348). Madrid: Aprendizaje Visor.Google Scholar
  55. Wagner, R. (1991). The fractal person. In M. Godelier & M. Strathern (Eds.), Big men and great men: Personifications of power in Melanesia. Cambridge: Maison des Sciences de l’Homme and Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  56. Werner, H. E., & Kaplan, B. (1956). The developmental approach to cognition: Its relevance to the psychological interpretation of anthropological and ethnolinguistic data. American Anthropologist, 58, 866–880.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2020

Authors and Affiliations

  • Danilo Silva Guimarães
    • 1
  1. 1.Institute of PsychologyUniversity of São PauloSão PauloBrazil

Personalised recommendations